Skip to main content

Paternal care effects outweigh gamete-mediated and personal environment effects during the transgenerational estimation of risk in fathead minnows

Abstract

Background

Individuals can estimate risk by integrating prenatal with postnatal and personal information, but the relative importance of different information sources during the transgenerational response is unclear. The estimated level of risk can be tested using the cognitive rule of risk allocation, which postulates that under consistent high-risk, antipredator efforts should decrease so that individual metabolic requirements can be satisfied. Here we conduct a comprehensive study on transgenerational risk transmission by testing whether risk allocation occurs across 12 treatments that consist of different maternal, paternal, parental care (including cross-fostering) and offspring risk environment combinations in the fathead minnow Pimephales promelas, a small cyprinid fish with alloparental care. In each risk environment, we manipulated perceived risk by continuously exposing individuals from birth onwards to conspecific alarm cues or a control water treatment. Using 2810 1-month old individuals, we then estimated shoaling behaviour prior to and subsequent to a novel mechanical predator disturbance.

Results

Overall, shoals estimating risk to be high were denser during the prestimulus period, and, following the risk allocation hypothesis, resumed normal shoaling densities faster following the disturbance. Treatments involving parental care consistently induced densest shoals and greatest levels of risk allocation. Although prenatal risk environments did not relate to paternal care intensity, greater care intensity induced more risk allocation when parents provided care for their own offspring as opposed to those that cross-fostered fry. In the absence of care, parental effects on shoaling density were relatively weak and personal environments modulated risk allocation only when parental risk was low.

Conclusions

Our study highlights the high relative importance of parental care as opposed to other information sources, and its function as a mechanism underlying transgenerational risk transmission.

Peer Review reports

Background

In a heterogeneous, ever-changing world where risk fluctuates temporally and spatially [1], individuals have to carefully balance antipredator strategies that are costly [2, 3] but increase survival [4, 5], with other fitness-enhancing activities such as foraging and mating [6]. Hence, to maximize individual fitness, individuals need to accurately estimate the level of risk to which they are exposed. For this purpose, they derive information from various risk cues that are present in their environment [7]. Additionally, they can obtain risk-related information from previous generations and integrate it through transgenerational effects. Transgenerational plasticity, which evolves only when environmental change is slower than generation time [8], or when future environmental change can be accurately predicted [9], is widespread [10]. Due to its relevance in allowing organisms to cope with climate change [11, 12], and in reducing populations’ extinction risk in a changing world [13], researchers have been pushing for a better understanding of this phenomenon [10, 12].

However, remarkably little is known about the relative importance of risk information from different sources during the transgenerational response. First, risk information can be transmitted to the next generation through prefertilization parental effects, i.e., risk-induced changes in maternal [14,15,16,17,18,19] or paternal gametes [16, 20] but the relative importance of maternal and paternal risk information remains an open question [16, 21]. Second, risk-related cues can be communicated to offspring through postfertilization parental effects such as altered parental care [22,23,24,25]. As prenatal and postnatal effects can be co-adapted [26], revealing their individual and interactive effects is important for understanding evolutionary processes [27,28,29]. Third, individuals can obtain personal risk information themselves, which can interact with parental information in different ways, dependent on the level of environmental autocorrelation, cue reliability and the accuracy of transgenerational inheritance [30,31,32]. Taken together, there is a clear need for more comprehensive studies that reveal the relative importance of risk-related prefertilization parental effects, postfertilization parental effects and personal risk information [21, 33, 34].

Here, we study the transmission and integration of risk from different sources across generations in the fathead minnow Pimephales promelas [35]. This cyprinid is a common prey fish widespread across North American rivers and lakes [36]. After shoaling as juveniles, adult males become territorial and provide intensive alloparental care to clutches [37]. Males also provide alloparental care to adopted eggs as doing so increases their attractiveness to females [38] but they do discriminate between own and adopted clutches, providing less care to the latter [39]. In this species, parental care consists of clutch defense from predators, and egg cleaning by constant rubbing and mouthing [36, 39]. P. promelas experiences heterogeneous predation risk across populations [40, 41] but within populations, the level of predation risk is often stable. This is because first, like other cyprinids, P. promelas is a fish without constitutive morphological defenses such as spiny fish rays or sturdy scales, making it vulnerable to a wide variety of general predators throughout ontogeny [42]. Second, they are a non-migratory species that often inhabit small lakes and ponds where predator presence is consistent over time [36]. Third, lifespans of predators often cover multiple P. promelas generations. For example, a common sympatric predator of minnows is the northern pike Esox lucius [43], whose maximum age ranges between 8 and 15 years [44]. This means that P. promelas with their 6-month generation time can be exposed to the same predator individuals for 16–30 generations, which suggests that predator presence can be reliably predicted across multiple generations. P. promelas is also a well-established model system for studying risk assessment [45, 46] and antipredator phenotypic plasticity [47,48,49], including transgenerational responses [50].

Using continuous exposure from birth onwards to either conspecific alarm cues (high-risk) or a water control (low-risk), in a split-clutch design, we crossed risk levels across maternal, paternal, parental care (including cross-fostering) and offspring environments, producing 12 different treatment combinations (Fig. 1). We also tracked parental care intensity (i.e., the average proportional time spent with the clutch) over the 4-day long care period in the four treatment combinations that involved parental care as a possible mechanism of transgenerational risk transmission. As we also consistently manipulated clutch size, either by removing some eggs to raise them in the absence of parental care, or by swapping clutches between parents for cross-fostering treatments, we also assessed to what extent the resulting proportional change in clutch size impacted parental care intensity.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Breeding design aiming to capture independent and cumulative impacts of paternal, maternal, parental care and personal environments on risk assessment in the offspring generation. Black fish indicate individuals that were from birth onwards exposed to conspecific alarm cues, white fish refer to the ones that instead received a water control treatment. Dashed lines indicate the two treatments that resulted from cross-fostering. The bottom row below the dotted line introduces the cumulative figures that represent the specific treatment combination. Sample sizes of families, individuals and shoals that were analyzed are stated throughout

Shoaling is a form of animal grouping well-known for its antipredator function in reducing risk for individual shoaling members [51, 52]. While the formation of dense shoals can additionally be beneficial in the context of social foraging, it is also associated with greater resource competition as well as higher disease transmission risk [53, 54]. Resulting from a trade-off between these costs and benefits, shoaling density increases as a function of the level of acute predation risk [54], which makes this trait well-suited to investigate risk assessment. That is why we studied shoaling density in 39-day old offspring. Furthermore, we studied the change in shoaling density following a novel predator disturbance, as it is established as another reliable method to reveal risk assessment in fish [48, 55]. Following the risk allocation hypothesis, which postulates lower investment into antipredator strategies under consistent high-risk so as to satisfy metabolic requirements [56, 57], P. promelas raised under continuous high background risk resume normal shoaling densities quicker after a novel disturbance [48]. In contrast, low-risk fish form denser shoals for a longer time after exposure to a novel stimulus [48]. Consequently, in our study we expect shoals that estimate risk to be high from either parental or personal information to form denser shoals during the prestimulus period and to show risk allocation, i.e., to quickly return to prestimulus shoaling densities. In contrast, shoals that estimate risk to be low should form loose shoals during the prestimulus period and denser shoals following the stimulus. We also consider the possible influence of other factors that may impact plastic responses such as maternal [58] or paternal condition [59] as well as other factors that may impact shoaling density such as the size of individuals within shoals as well as shoal homogeneity (i.e., variance in size within shoals) [details in 55].

Given that previous studies suggest transmission of risk through maternal gametes, paternal gametes and parental care, we predict that high-risk experienced by the mother, by the father and by the caring individuals all impact offspring risk assessment. Furthermore, given that gamete-mediated effects are generally weak across multiple meta-analyses [60, 61] and that Steiger [27] suggests parental care effects to far outweigh prenatal effects, our hypothesis is that the impact of the parental care environment will far outweigh the impact of gamete-mediated maternal and paternal effects alone. Additionally, following previous research on the impact of immediate risk of parental care intensity [22, 62, 63], we expect that high-risk caring parents that were lifelong exposed to high-risk prior to parental care will likewise provide less care to offspring. Furthermore, as parental care intensity has been shown to directly modulate antipredator responses in offspring [23, 24], we predict that parental care intensity may be a major mechanism that transmits risk across generations. Lastly, we expect that parentally transmitted information is integrated with the offspring environment, with the offspring environment having a greater impact on phenotypes as it is a more accurate predictor of future environmental conditions [8].

Results

Paternal care intensity

Three factors shaped variation in parental care intensity (Table 1). Paternal care intensity was influenced by the day of care (R2 = 0.020, 95% CI of R2: [0, 0.230], F1,31.733 =  6.035, p = 0.020) with ca. 10% increases in care over the 4-day period. Additionally, the proportional change in clutch size modified parental care intensity (R2 = 0.064 [0, 0.258], F1,39.334 = 5.730, p = 0.022). Males provided less care the more eggs we removed for our split-clutch “absence of parental care” treatments, and more care when the clutches that they received during cross-fostering contained more eggs, leading to up to ca. 40% differences in care intensity. Beyond these effects, paternal care intensity was best explained by whether the father provided care to its own clutch or to an adopted clutch (R2 = 0.132 [0.012, 0.316], F1,25.723 = 13.137, p = 0.001) rather than by risk treatment (see Additional file 1: Section 2). Offspring from clutches that received parental care from their original fathers (i.e., the biparental low-risk treatment that received care by a low-risk male and the biparental high-risk treatment that received care by a high-risk male) received ca. 12.4% more care than did offspring from treatments where clutches were adopted.

Table 1 Full and final linear mixed-effect models analysing variation in Pimephales promelas parental care intensity

Offspring morphology

Neither variation in average body size of shoals nor differences in within-shoal variation in body size (i.e., shoal homogeneity) were explained by any of the fixed effects or their interactions with risk treatment (all p ≥ 0.078, Additional file 1: Table S2).

Prestimulus shoaling density

Three factors explained variation in prestimulus shoaling density (Table 2). Shoals with larger average body sizes (R2 = 0.016, 95% CI of R2: [0, 0.160], F1272.434 = 5.012, p = 0.026) and fish in heterogeneous shoals (R2 = 0.036 [0.006, 0.180], F1279.452 = 10.990, p = 0.001) generally formed less dense shoals independent of the treatment (Table 2). In addition, risk treatment influenced shoaling density (R2 = 0.071 [0.044, 0.214], F11,128.894 = 2.270, p = 0.014, Fig. 2). Overall, largest average IIDs (i.e., least dense shoals) during the prestimulus period were observable in offspring that originated from a biparental high-risk environment and were cared for by low-risk males (dCohen = 0.607, 95% CI of dCohen: [0.148, 1.067], F1,278 = 5.766, p = 0.017). Within biparental high-risk shoals, low-risk caring males induced greater IIDs than high-risk caring males (dCohen = 0.692 [0.023, 1.360], t259.1 =  2.675, p = 0.008), and also greater ones than when the same offspring did not receive any parental care (dCohen = 0.690 [0.135, 1.245], t252.4 =  3.317, p = 0.001). Shortest average IIDs (i.e., the densest shoals) were observable in one of the no-care treatments, specifically in the one where offspring from high-risk parents were themselves exposed to high-risk (dCohen= 0.492 [0.044, 0.941], F1278 = 8.238, p = 0.004). This suggests additive effects of exposure to high-risk in both the parental and the offspring generation. Additionally, in the absence of care, paternal high-risk alone induced shoaling densities that were not significantly different from biparental high-risk (personal low-risk: dCohen =  0.209 [−0.324, 0.742], t267 = 0.920, p = 0.359; personal high-risk: dCohen = 0.228 [−0.415, 0.871], t267 = 1.376, p = 0.170). This was not the case for maternal high-risk, which induced less dense shoals than biparental high-risk treatments (personal low-risk: dCohen  = 0.585 [0.024, 1.146], t267 = 2.632, p = 0.009, personal high-risk: dCohen = 0.808 [0.167, 1.145], t267 = 3.431, p < 0.001). Personal risk had no significant effect on prestimulus shoaling densities (Fig. 2). Moreover, variation in parental care intensity was not related to variation in prestimulus shoaling density (Table 3).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Average (over a 10-min period) prestimulus interindividual distances (mean ± SE) of 39-day old fathead minnows showcasing (a) maternal × paternal × offspring risk interactions and (b) biparental × caring parent × presence of parental care risk interactions. In a, white dots represent offspring low-risk whereas black dots represent offspring high-risk environments. In b, offspring were always exposed to low-risk environments only; hence in b white dots represent the absence of parental care whereas black dots represent the presence of parental care. Within fish drawings, black fish indicate individuals that were from birth onwards exposed to conspecific alarm cues, white fish refer to the ones that instead received a water control treatment; for more detail see Fig. 1. Black circles and error bars represent treatments shown only once across figures, for every other color, same-colored markers refer to the same treatment across figures. Dashed error bars in b highlight the treatments that were cross-fostered. Different letters above and below bars indicate statistical differences at p < 0.05 according to post-hoc tests from linear mixed-effect models that also contained all remaining covariates (final model in Table 2)

Table 2 Full and final linear mixed-effect models analysing variation in Pimephales promelas shoaling density
Table 3 Full and final linear mixed-effect models analysing parental care-related factors on Pimephales promelas shoaling density

Change in shoaling density

Variation in the change in shoaling density was best explained by two factors (Table 2). First, heterogeneous shoals generally reduced offspring shoaling density following the stimulus to a greater extent (R2 = 0.029, 95% CI of R2: [0, 0.145], F1281 = 9.033, p = 0.003). Otherwise only the risk treatment modulated variation in the shoaling density changes (R2 = 0.101 [0.072, 0.218], F11281 = 2.911, p = 0.001, Table 2; Fig. 3, Additional file 1: Fig. S1). During model reduction, we had also discovered a significant risk treatment × average size within shoals interaction, but further analysis revealed this interaction to be inconsequential (see Additional file 1: Section 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Average change in shoaling densities (mean ± SE) of 39-day old fathead minnows as induced by a mechanical predator disturbance showcasing (a) maternal × paternal × offspring risk interactions and (b) biparental × caring parent × presence of parental care risk interactions. The dashed line is the zero referent and represents no change in shoaling density. In a, empty dots represent offspring low-risk whereas filled dots represent offspring high-risk environments. In b, offspring were always exposed to low-risk environments only; hence empty dots in b represent the absence of parental care whereas filled dots represent the presence of parental care. Within fish drawings, black fish indicate individuals that were from birth onwards exposed to conspecific alarm cues, white fish refer to the ones that instead received a water control treatment; for more detail see Fig. 1. Black circles and error bars represent treatments shown only once across figures, for every other color, same-colored markers refer to the same treatment across figures. Dashed error bars in b highlight the treatments that were cross-fostered. Different letters above bars indicate statistical differences at p < 0.05 according to post-hoc tests from linear mixed-effect models that also contained all remaining covariates (final model in Table 2). Raw values are plotted in Additional file 1: Fig. S1

Overall, largest average increases in IIDs (i.e., patterns of risk allocation) were observable in offspring that originated from a biparental high-risk environment and were cared for by high-risk males (dCohen =  0.591, 95% CI of dCohen: [0.121, 1.061], F1279 = 6.114, χ2 = 5.914, p = 0.014), suggesting additive effects of parental and caring parent risk (Fig. 3, Additional file 1: Fig. S1). Parental care by high-risk males induced similar levels of risk allocation in biparental low-risk offspring as in biparental high-risk offspring (dCohen = 0.278 [−0.393, 0.949], t268 = 0.846, p = 0.399). At the same time, without any other sources of risk information involved, parental care by high-risk males clearly induced greater levels of risk allocation than low-risk care (dCohen = 0.757 [0.056, 1.458], t284 = 2.563, p = 0.011), than maternal high-risk (high personal risk: dCohen =  0.763 [0.097, 1.429], t268 = −2.386, p = 0.018; low personal risk: dCohen =  0.634 [−0.050, 1.318], t268 = 1.923, p = 0.056) and than personal high-risk (dCohen = 0.707 [0.036, 1.378], t269 = −2.051, p = 0.041).

Largest decreases in IIDs as induced by the stimulus were observable in offspring from a biparental low-risk environment that were cared for by low-risk males (dCohen= 0.504 [0.023, 0.985], F1,279 =  5.759, p = 0.017). Further, the two care types (taking care of own or adopted eggs) differed in the extent that average parental care intensity impacted on the change in shoaling density (Table 3); when own eggs were cared for, both higher care intensities (R2 = 0.227 [0, 0.474], F1,37 = 13.600, p < 0.001, Fig. 4a) and high-risk experience in the caring male (R2 = 0.138 [0, 0.400], F1,37 = 8.423, p = 0.006) induced greater levels of risk allocation, but this effect was absent when eggs were adopted (care intensity: R2 = 0.009 [0, 0.321], F1,18.552 = 0.235, p = 0.633; risk treatment: R2= 0.027 [0, 0.347], F1,18.516 =  0.721, p = 0.407, Fig. 4b).

In the absence of parental care, biparental high-risk induced greater risk allocation than offspring from low-risk parents that experienced personal high-risk (personal low-risk: dCohen = 0.611 [0.068, 1.154], t268 = 2.443, p = 0.015; personal high-risk: dCohen = 0.489 [−0.144, 1.122], t268 = 2.068, p = 0.040) as well as than maternal-only high-risk exposure (personal low-risk: dCohen = 0.553 [−0.007, 1.113], t268 = 2.263, p = 0.025; personal high-risk: dCohen = 0.540 [−0.087, 1.167], t268 = 2.405, p = 0.017). Furthermore, paternal high-risk alone again generated offspring with similar patterns of risk allocation to biparental high-risk (personal high-risk: dCohen = 0.454 [−0.196, 1.103], t268 = 1.624, p = 0.106; personal low-risk: dCohen =  0.277 [−0.257, 0.811], t268 = 0.846, p = 0.399). Maternal high-risk alone induced intermediate responses and consequently differed from biparental high-risk (all p ≤ 0.025) but not from either biparental low-risk (all p ≥ 0.054) or paternal high-risk (all p ≥ 0.228). Different offspring environments impacted shoaling density only within the biparental low-risk treatment (dCohen = 0.534 [−0.014, 1.082], t265 = 2.101, p = 0.037). Here, high-risk offspring reduced their IIDs in response to the mechanical stimulus whereas low-risk offspring increased them. This suggests that personal environments have a lower impact when high-risk is present in the parental generation (Fig. 3, Additional file 1: Fig. S1).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Relationship between average paternal care intensity and the change in interindividual density following the mechanical predator disturbance for (a) own and (b) adopted offspring from the opposing risk treatment, respectively. Filled dots visualize instances where high-risk males provided care, empty dots instead refer to care by low-risk males. Shaded areas (95% confidence intervals) and regression lines for the treatment-independent correlation (thick solid line) were estimated from mixed-effect models with treatment (low-risk/high-risk) as covariate and caring individual ID nested in shoal family origin as random intercept. Regression lines for treatment-specific correlations (low-risk care: dashed lines, high-risk care: dot-dashed lines) are shown without confidence intervals to avoid visual clutter. The dotted line is the zero referent and represents no change in shoaling density

Discussion

Across risk treatments, the involvement of parental care consistently induced both highest and lowest shoaling density responses. Prior to any stimulus, offspring from high-risk parents that received care by low-risk males formed the least dense shoals, which is a typical low-risk response (Fig. 2). Furthermore, additive effects from the same biparental and parental care risk levels induced stronger responses that follow the predictions by the risk allocation hypothesis: a quick return to prestimulus shoaling densities after a novel mechanical stimulus in individuals from high-risk treatment combinations but not in low-risk ones (Fig. 3). Paternal care intensity was the highest if the offspring they cared for was their own, and care intensity showed a weak positive correlation with clutch age as well as with the proportional change in clutch size due to experimental manipulation (Table 1). Furthermore, care intensity was positively correlated with the level of risk allocation when parents took care of their own eggs (Fig. 4). In the absence of parental care, maternal effects, which were subtle to non-existent, were outweighed by paternal effects, but biparental high-risk induced the densest shoals and the greatest extent of risk allocation (Figs. 2 and 3). The impact of personal risk environments was generally low and only statistically significant when no risk was present in the parental generation (Figs. 2 and 3). Generally, the present study highlights the high importance of parental care relative to gamete-mediated effects in shaping offspring shoaling density and risk allocation patterns.

Treatments involving parental care were consistently the ones that induced greatest shoaling density responses and these outweighed gamete-induced effects for both prestimulus density and the change in shoaling density (Figs. 2 and 3). This is in accordance with multiple meta-analyses that question the adaptiveness of gamete-mediated maternal and paternal effects alone [60, 61] and with the suggestion that effects of parental care outweigh prenatal effects [27]. The general direction of parental care risk environments (i.e., while not always being statistically significant, high-risk care induced at least slightly denser shoals and risk allocation whereas low-risk care induced the opposite pattern; Figs. 2b and 3b) appears to support the hypothesis that prenatal high-risk and low-risk in the caring individuals induces putatively adaptive responses typical for high and low-risk, respectively. While a larger sample size may be necessary to reveal a significant impact of these effects across all risk treatments that involved parental care, a similar pattern was previously observed in response to high and low immediate risk during care [23].

Variation in parental care intensity was to a small extent explained by whether eggs were adopted or not, proportional changes in clutch size and the age of the eggs (Table 1, see Additional file 1: Section 4 for discussion of these effects). However, even when statistically controlling for these slightly confounding factors, there was no general effect of prenatal caring parent risk on parental care intensity (Table 1), contrasting other studies reporting that immediate high-risk environments induce lower parental care intensities [22, 62]. This lower than expected impact of prenatal risk environments on parental care intensity may result from us using a P. promelas population that has been kept in the laboratory dating back to 1985, and possibly earlier (see Additional file 1: Section 1 for more detail). Such laboratory populations may not have evolved predator-induced plasticity in parental care intensity due to the absence of fluctuating risk [sensu 64]. Consequently, risk-induced differences in parental care intensity are unlikely to be the main mechanism for the transmission of risk information across generations. Instead, because fish embryos are capable of detecting environmental cues from within their eggs across taxa [65, 66], caring males may transfer prenatal risk to offspring through mechanisms that are not correlated with care intensity such as the release of disturbance cues [67], steroid hormones [68] or mechanosensory cues [69] following the theory that adult behaviours may mimic environmental cues experienced by the parents themselves [70].

In addition, when fathers took care of their own offspring, independent of prenatal risk levels, high levels of paternal care intensity also induced greater offspring risk allocation (Fig. 4a). This result matches previous observations across fish taxa that parental care intensity directly modulates antipredator responses in offspring [23, 24]. The proximate mechanism here may be glucocorticoid receptor methylation differences that are associated with variation in parental care intensity [71, 72]. As perceived predation risk elevates glucocorticoid levels in prey [73, 74], care-induced differences in associated receptor expression are likely to affect offspring responses to perceived predation risk. That care intensity was not correlated with offspring risk allocation in cross-fostered clutches (Fig. 4b) may either be a consequence of environmental mismatches between parental and parental care environment or alternatively a by-product of cross-fostering negatively impacting parental care intensities. Future studies are clearly required to disentangle these two possibilities.

Within treatments that did not involve parental care, paternal risk effects mostly outweighed maternal effects in terms of the adaptiveness of the response for a high-risk environment for both prestimulus shoaling density and the change in shoaling density (Figs. 2a and 3a), suggesting the presence of sex-specific transgenerational plasticity [21]. The general direction of paternal effects mostly outweighing maternal effects contrasts other studies across taxa suggesting that usually maternal effects outweigh paternal effects [snails: 75, birds: 76, fish: 77]. Our observation is still in accordance with many studies highlighting the relevance of paternal effects [78, 79], and a recent meta-analysis suggesting that paternal effect sizes are on average larger than maternal ones [61]. This could be because paternal sperm methylomes are known to be inherited to offspring unaltered whereas maternal methylation patterns experience substantial reorganization [80, 81]. Alternatively, paternal environmental information may be more reliable than maternal information [21], mainly because caring male P. promelas are more likely to share their environment with offspring compared to females which disperse after egg deposition. However, we cannot exclude the possibility that the few minutes to hours that fathers took care of the embryos immediately after egg deposition might already be sufficient to transmit some information about the paternal environment via parental care, making the presumably paternal gamete-mediated effects possible by-products of paternal care effects. Furthermore, paternal high-risk did not induce different effects from personal experience with high-risk (Figs. 2a and 3a), in accordance with a previous stickleback study [82]. In the absence of care, biparental high-risk consistently induced the on average densest shoals (Fig. 2a) and the on average greatest extent of risk allocation (Fig. 3a), suggesting that maternal and paternal perceived risk have additive effects on offspring, in accordance with some previous stickleback studies [83] but contrasting other stickleback research suggesting non-additive effects [16, 84]. These additive effects between maternal and paternal environments are then further additively exacerbated by high-risk parental care (Figs. 2b and 3b), which, following the theory of co-adaptation of prenatal and postnatal effects [26], helps to maximize risk allocation in high-risk environments.

Parental environments sometimes also affected the impact of personal environments on shoaling density. Although during the prestimulus period, high personal risk appears to slightly increase shoaling density mostly independent of parental risk levels (Fig. 2a), which is an adaptive response [51, 52], these effects were not statistically significant. The only significant effect of high personal risk emerged following the stimulus (Fig. 3a), matching the finding that some level of immediate risk is required to reveal the effects of background risk exposure [48, 75]. High personal risk appears to reduce risk allocation to a greater extent the less information about high-risk is present in the parental generation, culminating in a significantly reduced risk allocation only when both parents were derived from a low-risk environment (Fig. 3a). This result is in accordance with information theory that predicts decreasing confidence in information when different sources provide conflicting cues [85]. Accordingly, mismatches between parental and personal risk communicate that background risk levels are not consistent, an instance where risk allocation is not adaptive. Compared to personal risk, parental risk levels were more important in modulating risk allocation. This result is in accordance with Bayesian updating theory, which predicts a greater reliance on parental risk information especially during juvenile life-stages, as parents have had the opportunity to repeatedly sample the environment over longer temporal scales [86]. By relying more on parental information, juveniles can effectively avoid the danger of overestimating spurious fluctuations in risk, especially in species such as P. promelas where predator presence is putatively highly predictable over multiple generations. Furthermore, that we did not observe significantly better-adapted phenotypes when both parental and personal risk were high (Figs. 2a and 3a) is in accordance with the reported benefits of environmental matching across generations being weak in general [60].

Lastly, independent of treatment, large average fish size within shoals and heterogeneous shoals had small negative effects on shoal densities; this result is largely independent of measuring these effects in absolute or relative units (Additional file 1: Table S3). This follows the theory that predation risk is higher for smaller fish [87], and that smaller fish should avoid shoaling with larger conspecifics due to food competition [88].

Conclusions

Taken together, our study highlights for the first time the high relative importance of risk transmission through parental care as opposed to other sources of risk information within and across generations. Furthermore, the widespread neglect of parental care effects during transgenerational research may thus contribute to small observed effect sizes [60, 61]. Consequently, we hope that our study encourages more research on the exact mechanism for risk transmission during parental care and a greater focus on parental care as a mechanism underlying presumably inherited gene regulation.

Methods

Experimental fish

In collaboration with the University of Saskatchewan’s Aquatic Toxicology Research Facility (ATRF), Canada, we bred adult P. promelas sourced from their own predator-free laboratory stock population to generate the parental fish for our experiment [47,48,49]. Eggs were collected after deposition, moved to the University of Saskatchewan’s RJF Center for Aquatic Ecology and raised using a split-clutch design based on exposing individuals continuously from birth onwards to either conspecific alarm cues [which reliably signals high-risk and induces typical antipredator phenotypes across taxa, including fathead minnows, see 48, 67, 89] or a distilled water control (low-risk). In contrast to predator cues, alarm cues are innately recognized [90] and no habituation occurs even after repeated exposure [91]. As the fish in the ATRF were sexually mature, which reduces alarm cue efficacy [92, 93], alarm cue donors were non-reproductive conspecifics collected from the University of Saskatchewan’s Feedlot Pond (52°09’23.4"N, 106°37’04.5"W). Alarm cues from this population have been proven multiple times to be recognizable to conspecifics as high-risk cues [45, 47,48,49, 94]. In the next generation, we used our parental individuals to generate a total of 12 treatment combinations using a similar set-up (Fig. 1). First, in the absence of parental care, risk levels were crossed across maternal, paternal and offspring environments in a full-factorial 2 × 2 × 2 design. Additionally, to investigate the relative importance of parental care, we further split some of the previously used clutches so as to generate a 2 × 2 factorial design that crossed (genetic) biparental risk with caring parent risk; offspring consistently grew up in a low-risk environment here. In the two treatments where caring parent risk was mismatched with biparental risk levels, we cross-fostered offspring; although we intended to do so for all four parental care treatments, the number of pairs from the same parental environment combination that spawned concurrently was insufficient. At the same time, space limitations did not allow us to investigate effects of parental care for other maternal, parental and offspring risk environment combinations. For additional details of the raising process see Additional file 1: Section 1.

Parental care

To assess variation in parental care intensity in the four treatments that involved parental care, we recorded caring male behaviour every day until hatching (4 days) for a period of 10 min between 1600 and 1900 h, before feeding, using a camera (C922x Pro Stream, Logitech, Suzhou, China).

Shoaling assays

We used 39-day old fish to run the shoaling assays as described in Meuthen et al. [48]. In brief, 21.4 × 21.4 × 21.8 cm white pails are filled with 500 ml water (temperature 20.0 ± 0.1 °C), resulting in a shallow water depth of 1.09 cm, which allows evaluating between-fish distances in two dimensions [95] and at the same time represents a naturally realistic context for shoals of juvenile P. promelas as they typically inhabit shallow water habitats [40]. A predator disturbance stimulus could be provided through a wooden apparatus that provided a hit of 0.108 joules to the bucket [48]; this novel mechanical stimulus reliably induces typical antipredator responses [48, 55]. Trials were video-taped (C922x Pro Stream). Experimental shoals were gently introduced in the set-up, this was followed by a 10-min acclimation period. Afterwards, the experiment started with a 10-min prestimulus period, followed by the delivery of the mechanical predator disturbance stimulus, and a 10 min poststimulus period. In total, we tested 281 shoals of 10 fish each (see Fig. 1 for details). We had originally tested 10 additional shoals but had to exclude them due to issues with nearby construction noise [which affects antipredator responses, see 96], power outages, unexpected mortality immediately before the trial, or procedural errors by the experimenter.

Data analysis

Parental phenotypes

As maternal [58] or paternal condition [59] may influence antipredator plasticity, for every parental individual, we calculated condition indices according to Bolger and Connolly [97]: [100 * mass (g)]/standard length (cm)x, with x = 3.043 as assessed by regressing log10 weight against log10 standard length over all parental individuals. Neither maternal (Wilcoxon rank-sum test, W = 222.5, p = 0.754) nor paternal condition (Wilcoxon rank-sum test, W = 214.5, p = 0.705) differed between parental risk treatments.

Parental care

To standardize the time from moving the camera in front of the tank (i.e., a visual disturbance), we analyzed only the last 5 min of each recording, an established time period across minnow parental care studies [98]. We found that male egg manipulation such as rubbing and mouthing occurs almost constantly when males were in the vicinity of the clutch (often more than once per second, preventing repeatability when counting), hence in every recording we measured the relative time the male spent within one standard length of the eggs (inside the breeding tile) as is done as a typical measure of parental care intensity across taxa [99]. Four recordings per clutch were analyzed, in total 160 videos from 20 different caretakers that cared for 40 clutches. For every clutch, we calculated the average relative time that the male spent with the clutch as a proxy for average parental care intensity. Furthermore, as variation in parental investment between treatments may reflect in the slope of parental care over time [99], for each clutch we performed robust regression by calculating Theil-Sen-Siegel slopes of the care given over the 4-day period; using such repeated medians to estimate slopes is robust to outliers [100].

Size measurements

To assess the size of fish within shoals, we selected a frame from each video where fish movement was minimal and where fish bodies were as straight as possible. We extracted this frame using VirtualDub (A. Lee 1998–2012; version 1.10.4) and measured fish sizes (total length, i.e., from the tip of the snout to the end of the tail fin) in ImageJ (Rasband 1997–2018, US National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA). By using known pail size as a reference, we converted the fish size in pixels into millimeters. For each shoal, we first calculated the average body size. Then, as a proxy for shoal homogeneity, which is known to impact shoaling behavior [55, 101], we also calculated coefficients of variation for each shoal (CV, dividing the standard deviation by the mean).

Shoaling distance measurements

To assess shoaling distances, we extracted 1 image every 30 s from the video file, resulting in 40 images per shoal (20 pre- and 20 post-stimulus images). We then measured interindividual distances (IIDs), which are more precise than nearest-neighbor distances when shoal size is kept constant across trials [102] using tpsDig2 2.30 (F. James Rohlf, Stony Brook University, USA) and CoordGen 8 (Integrated Morphometrics Package Suite, H. David Sheets, Canisius College, USA). As 10 fish were present in each image, 45 IIDs could be extracted from every frame. In total, from 281 shoals of 10 fish each (18–43 shoals per risk treatment, see Fig. 1), we generated 11,240 images and measured 505,800 IIDs. First, we averaged IIDs for each frame, and then for each shoal, we calculated mean IIDs over the 10 min prestimulus period and the following 10 min poststimulus period. Within-shoal variation in IIDs over frames was small and did not differ between treatments (mean ± SD coefficient of variation along with Kruskal-Wallis tests; overall 0.215 ± 0.060, χ2 = 9.501, df = 11, p = 0.576; prestimulus 0.211 ± 0.065, χ2 = 8.443, df = 11, p = 0.673; poststimulus 0.214 ± 0.065, χ2 = 13.331, df = 11, p = 0.272). Afterwards, we also calculated the average change in IID that was induced by the mechanical stimulus (post-pre). Assessing the change in IID as proportional change from the prestimulus baseline (i.e., post-pre/pre) in preliminary analyses did not qualitatively or quantitatively affect our results. All data from this study are deposited in Additional file 2.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted with R 4.0.3 [103], accompanied by the packages lme4 1.1-26 [104], lmerTest 3.1-3 [105], emmeans 1.5.4 [106], effsize 0.8.1 [107] and partR2 0.9.1 [108]. We applied linear mixed-effect models with family identity as a random intercept using maximum likelihood parameter estimation throughout; full models were then subject to stepwise reduction using backward elimination procedures with Satterthwaite approximations (retaining random factors) until only significant fixed effects remained in final models. Quantile-quantile plots of model residuals were inspected to ascertain that parametric assumptions were satisfied. In the final models, we calculated partial R² values and Cohen’s d along with 95% confidence intervals to visualize effect sizes. First, we analyzed whether risk treatment, care type (own/adopted), the day of care, final clutch size (i.e., the number of eggs that were cared for), the change in clutch size or any of their interactions with risk treatment explained variation in parental care intensity (Yeo-Johnson transformed), with day of care as a random slope and caring male ID as nested random intercept. Afterwards, we aimed to reveal whether risk treatment, original clutch size (i.e., the total number of eggs in the clutch they were derived from), parental condition or their interactions with risk treatment explained variation in the average size within shoals or within-shoal variation in body size (i.e., shoal homogeneity). Third, we assessed whether risk treatment, average size within shoals, shoal homogeneity, parental condition or any of their interactions with risk treatment explained variation in prestimulus IID or the change in IID. Fourth, for the four treatments that involved parental care, we assessed to what degree variation in parental care intensity or in the slope of parental care explained variation in both prestimulus IID or the change in IID (with caring male ID as nested random intercept). When risk treatment effects were significant in the final models, we conducted pairwise post-hoc analyses. Families were not shared among all treatment combinations, hence we present the results from post-hoc analyses with family identity as a random effect only where it is appropriate (in 14 of 66 pairwise comparisons); otherwise, we provide data from post-hoc models without random effects.

Availability of data and materials

The data and materials supporting the conclusions of this article are included within the article and its Additional files 1, 2.

References

  1. Sih A, Ziemba R, Harding KC. New insights on how temporal variation in predation risk shapes prey behavior. Trends Ecol Evol. 2000;15:3–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Janssens L, Stoks R. Predation risk causes oxidative damage in prey. Biol Lett. 2013;9:20130350.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Murren CJ, Auld JR, Callahan H, Ghalambor CK, Handelsman CA, Heskel MA, Kingsolver JG, Maclean HJ, Masel J, Maughan H, et al: Constraints on the evolution of phenotypic plasticity: limits and costs of phenotype and plasticity. Heredity. 2015; 115:293–301.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Hoverman JT, Relyea RA. Survival trade-offs associated with inducible defences in snails: the roles of multiple predators and developmental plasticity. Funct Ecol. 2009;23:1179–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Robinson BW, Januszkiewicz AJ, Koblitz JC. Survival benefits and divergence of predator-induced behavior between pumpkinseed sunfish ecomorphs. Behav Ecol. 2008;19:263–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Sih A. Prey uncertainty and the balancing of antipredator and feeding needs. Am Nat. 1992;139:1052–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Wisenden BD. Olfactory assessment of predation risk in the aquatic environment. Philos Trans R Soc B. 2000;355:1205–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Kuijper B, Hoyle RB. When to rely on maternal effects and when on phenotypic plasticity? Evolution. 2015;69:950–68.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Leimar O, McNamara JM: The evolution of transgenerational integration of information in heterogeneous environments. Am Nat. 2015; 185:E55-E69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Salinas S, Brown Simon C, Mangel M, Munch Stephan B: Non-genetic inheritance and changing environments. Non-Genet Inherit. 2013; 1:38–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Meylan S, Miles DB, Clobert J. Hormonally mediated maternal effects, individual strategy and global change. Philos Trans R Soc B. 2012;367:1647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Donelan SC, Hellmann JK, Bell AM, Luttbeg B, Orrock JL, Sheriff MJ, Sih A: Transgenerational plasticity in human-altered environments. Trends Ecol Evol. 2020; 35:115–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Garbutt JS, Little TJ, Hoyle A. Maternal effects on offspring consumption can stabilize fluctuating predator-prey systems. Proc R Soc B. 2015;282:20152173.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Cattelan S, Herbert-Read J, Panizzon P, Devigili A, Griggio M, Pilastro A, Morosinotto C: Maternal predation risk increases offspring’s exploration but does not affect schooling behavior. Behav Ecol. 2020; 31:1207–1217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. McGhee KE, Barbosa AJ, Bissell K, Darby NA, Foshee S: Maternal stress during pregnancy affects activity, exploration and potential dispersal of daughters in an invasive fish. Anim Behav. 2021; 171:41–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Hellmann JK, Bukhari SA, Deno J, Bell AM. Sex-specific transgenerational plasticity in threespined sticklebacks. bioRxiv. 2020:763862.

  17. Storm JJ, Lima SL. Mothers forewarn offspring about predators: a transgenerational maternal effect on behavior. Am Nat. 2010;175:382–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Giesing ER, Suski CD, Warner RE, Bell AM. Female sticklebacks transfer information via eggs: effects of maternal experience with predators on offspring. Proc R Soc B. 2011;278:1753–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. McGhee KE, Pintor LM, Suhr EL, Bell AM. Maternal exposure to predation risk decreases offspring antipredator behaviour and survival in threespined stickleback. Funct Ecol. 2012;26:932–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Chen E, Zielinski C, Deno J, Singh R, Bell AM, Hellmann JK. The specificity of sperm-mediated paternal effects in threespined sticklebacks. bioRxiv. 2020:135152.

  21. Bell AM, Hellmann JK. An integrative framework for understanding the mechanisms and multigenerational consequences of transgenerational plasticity. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 2019;50:97–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. McGhee KE, Feng S, Leasure S, Bell AM. A female’s past experience with predators affects male courtship and the care her offspring will receive from their father. Proc R Soc B. 2015;282:20151840.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Stein LR, Bell AM. Paternal programming in sticklebacks. Anim Behav. 2014;95:165–71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. McGhee KE, Bell AM. Paternal care in a fish: epigenetics and fitness enhancing effects on offspring anxiety. Proc R Soc B. 2014;281:20141146.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Champagne FA. Epigenetic mechanisms and the transgenerational effects of maternal care. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2008;29:386–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Lock JE, Smiseth PT, Moore PJ, Moore AJ. Coadaptation of prenatal and postnatal maternal effects. Am Nat. 2007;170:709–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Steiger S. Bigger mothers are better mothers: disentangling size-related prenatal and postnatal maternal effects. Proc R Soc B. 2013;280:20131225.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Alonzo SH. Maternal effects likely coevolve with plasticity in male care: a comment on Paquet and Smiseth. Behav Ecol. 2016;27:696–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Paquet M, Smiseth PT. Maternal effects as a mechanism for manipulating male care and resolving sexual conflict over care. Behav Ecol. 2016;27:685–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Räsänen K, Kruuk LEB. Maternal effects and evolution at ecological time-scales. Funct Ecol. 2007;21:408–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Luquet É, Tariel J. Offspring reaction norms shaped by parental environment: interaction between within- and trans-generational plasticity of inducible defenses. BMC Evol Biol. 2016;16:209.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. McNamara JM, Dall SRX, Hammerstein P, Leimar O. Detection vs. selection: integration of genetic, epigenetic and environmental cues in fluctuating environments. Ecol Lett. 2016;19:1267–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Tariel J, Plénet S, Luquet É. Transgenerational plasticity in the context of predator-prey interactions. Front Ecol Evol. 2020;8:319.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Donelson JM, Salinas S, Munday PL, Shama LNS. Transgenerational plasticity and climate change experiments: where do we go from here? Glob Change Biol. 2018;24:13–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Rafinesque CS. Ichthyologia Ohiensis—natural history of the fishes inhabiting the river Ohio and its tributary streams Lexington. Kentucky: W. G. Hunt; 1820.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Scott WB, Crossman EJ. Freshwater fishes of Canada Ontario. Oakville: Galt House Publications Ltd; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  37. McMillan VE, Smith RJF. Agonistic and reproductive behaviour of the fathead minnow (Pimephales promelas Rafinesque). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 1974;34:25–58.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Unger LM, Sargent RC. Allopaternal care in the fathead minnow, Pimephales promelas: females prefer males with eggs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1988;23:27–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Sargent RC. Allopaternal care in the fathead minnow, Pimephales promelas: stepfathers discriminate against their adopted eggs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1989;25:379–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Mathis A, Chivers DP, Smith RJF. Population differences in responses of fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas) to visual and chemical stimuli from predators. Ethology. 1993;93:31–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Chivers DP, Brown GE, Smith RJF. The evolution of chemical alarm signals: Attracting predators benefits alarm signal senders. Am Nat. 1996;148:649–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Hoogland R, Morris D, Tinbergen N. The spines of sticklebacks (Gasterosteus and Pygosteus) as means of defence against predators (Perca and Esox). Behaviour. 1956;10:205–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Mann RHK. The annual food consumption and prey preferences of pike (Esox lucius) in the River Frome, Dorset. J Anim Ecol. 1982;51:81–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Clark CF, Steinbach F. Observations on the age and growth of the northern pike, Esox lucius L., in East Harbor, Ohio. Ohio J Sci. 1959;59:129–34.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Chivers DP, Smith RJF. The role of experience and chemical alarm signaling in predator recognition by fathead minnows, Pimephales promelas. J Fish Biol. 1994;44:273–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Carreau-Green ND, Mirza RS, Martinez ML, Pyle GG. The ontogeny of chemically mediated antipredator responses of fathead minnows Pimephales promelas. J Fish Biol. 2008;73:2390–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Meuthen D, Ferrari MCO, Lane T, Chivers DP. Predation risk induces age- and sex-specific morphological plastic responses in the fathead minnow Pimephales promelas. Sci Rep. 2019;9:15378.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Meuthen D, Ferrari MCO, Lane T, Chivers DP. High background risk induces risk allocation rather than generalized neophobia in the fathead minnow. Behav Ecol. 2019;30:1416–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Meuthen D, Ferrari MCO, Lane T, Chivers DP. Plasticity of boldness: high perceived risk eliminates a relationship between boldness and body size in fathead minnows. Anim Behav. 2019;147:25–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Crane AL, Meuthen D, Thapa H, Ferrari MCO, Brown GE: Early-life and parental predation risk shape fear acquisition in adult minnows. Anim Cogn. 2021; 24 471–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Magurran AE. The adaptive significance of schooling as an antipredator defense in fish. Ann Zool Fenn. 1990; 27:51–66.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Hamilton WD. Geometry for the selfish herd. J Theor Biol. 1971;31:295–311.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Krause J, Ruxton GD. Living in groups. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Pitcher TJ, Parrish JK. Functions of shoaling behavior in teleosts. In: Pitcher TJ, editor. Behaviour of teleost fishes. London: Chapman & Hall; 1993. p. 363–439.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  55. Meuthen D, Baldauf SA, Bakker TCM, Thünken T. Predator-induced neophobia in juvenile cichlids. Oecologia. 2016;181:947–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Lima SL, Bednekoff PA. Temporal variation in danger drives antipredator behavior: the predation risk allocation hypothesis. Am Nat. 1999;153:649–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Ferrari MCO, Sih A, Chivers DP. The paradox of risk allocation: a review and prospectus. Anim Behav. 2009;78:579–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Bennett AM, Murray DL. Maternal body condition influences magnitude of anti-predator response in offspring. Proc R Soc B. 2014;281:20141806.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Kelly PW, Pfennig DW, Pfennig KS. A condition-dependent male sexual signal predicts adaptive predator-induced plasticity in offspring. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2021;75:28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Uller T, Nakagawa S, English S. Weak evidence for anticipatory parental effects in plants and animals. J Evol Biol. 2013;26:2161–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Sánchez-Tójar A, Lagisz M, Moran NP, Nakagawa S, Noble DWA, Reinhold K. The jury is still out regarding the generality of adaptive ‘transgenerational’ effects. Ecol Lett. 2020;23:1715–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Arundell KL, Wedell N, Dunn AM. The impact of predation risk and of parasitic infection on parental care in brooding crustaceans. Anim Behav. 2014;96:97–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Oteyza JC, Mouton JC, Martin TE. Adult survival probability and body size affect parental risk-taking across latitudes. Ecol Lett. 2021;24:20–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Stein LR, Bell AM. The role of variation and plasticity in parental care during the adaptive radiation of three-spine sticklebacks. Evolution. 2019;73:1037–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Atherton JA, McCormick MI. Kin recognition in embryonic damselfishes. Oikos. 2017;126:1062–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Kusch RC, Chivers DP. The effects of crayfish predation on phenotypic and life-history variation in fathead minnows. Can J Zool. 2004;82:917–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Chivers DP, Brown GE, Ferrari MCO. The evolution of alarm substances and disturbance cues in aquatic animals. In: Brönmark C, Hansson LA, editors. Chemical ecology in aquatic systems. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012. p. 127–39.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  68. Scott AP, Ellis T. Measurement of fish steroids in water-a review. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2007;153:392–400.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Manassa RP, McCormick MI, Chivers DP, Ferrari MCO. Social learning of predators in the dark: understanding the role of visual, chemical and mechanical information. Proc R Soc B. 2013;280:20130720.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Ledón-Rettig CC, Richards CL, Martin LB. Epigenetics for behavioral ecologists. Behav Ecol. 2013;24:311–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Weaver ICG, Cervoni N, Champagne FA, D’Alessio AC, Sharma S Jr, Dymov S, Szyf S, Meaney M. Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:847–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Turecki G, Meaney MJ. Effects of the social environment and stress on glucocorticoid receptor gene methylation: a systematic review. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79:87–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Humphrey EA, Crespi E, Travis J. Under pressure: short- and long-term response to predation varies in two populations of a live-bearing fish. Ethology. 2020;126:463–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Dulude-de Broin F, Hamel S, Mastromonaco GF, Côté SD. Predation risk and mountain goat reproduction: Evidence for stress-induced breeding suppression in a wild ungulate. Funct Ecol. 2020;34:1003–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Tariel J, Luquet É, Plénet S. Interactions between maternal, paternal, developmental, and immediate environmental effects on anti-predator behavior of the snail Physa acuta. Front Ecol Evol. 2020;8:591074.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Romero-Haro AA, Alonso-Alvarez C. Oxidative stress experienced during early development influences the offspring phenotype. Am Nat. 2020;196:704–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Jonsson B, Jonsson N. Trans-generational maternal effect: temperature influences egg size of the offspring in Atlantic salmon Salmo salar. J Fish Biol. 2016;89:1482–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. McCarthy DM, Morgan TJ Jr., Lowe SE, Williamson MJ, Spencer TJ, Biederman J, Bhide PG. Nicotine exposure of male mice produces behavioral impairment in multiple generations of descendants. PLoS Biol. 2018;16:e2006497.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  79. Guillaume AS, Monro K, Marshall DJ. Transgenerational plasticity and environmental stress: do paternal effects act as a conduit or a buffer? Funct Ecol. 2016;30:1175–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Hackett JA, Surani MA. Beyond DNA: programming and inheritance of parental methylomes. Cell. 2013;153:737–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Potok ME, Nix DA, Parnell TJ, Cairns BR. Reprogramming the maternal zebrafish genome after fertilization to match the paternal methylation pattern. Cell. 2013;153:759–72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Stein LR, Bukhari SA, Bell AM. Personal and transgenerational cues are nonadditive at the phenotypic and molecular level. Nat Ecol Evol. 2018;2:1306–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Lehto WR, Tinghitella RM. Joint maternal and paternal stress increases the cortisol in their daughters’ eggs. Evol Ecol Res. 2019;20:133–44.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Lehto WR, Tinghitella RM. Predator-induced maternal and paternal effects independently alter sexual selection. Evolution. 2020;74:404–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Hebets EA, Papaj DR. Complex signal function: developing a framework of testable hypotheses. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2005;57:197–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Stamps JA, Bell AM. Combining information from parental and personal experiences: Simple processes generate diverse outcomes. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0250540.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Reznick DN, Butler MJ, Rodd FH, Ross P. Life-history evolution in guppies (Poecilia reticulata).6. Differential mortality as a mechanism for natural selection. Evolution. 1996;50:1651–60.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Ranta E, Peuhkuri N, Laurila A. A theoretical exploration of antipredatory and foraging factors promoting phenotype-assorted fish schools. Ecoscience. 1994;1:99–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Chivers DP, Smith RJF. Chemical alarm signalling in aquatic predator-prey systems: a review and prospectus. Ecoscience. 1998;5:338–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Brown GE. Learning about danger: chemical alarm cues and local risk assessment in prey fishes. Fish Fisheries. 2003;4:227–34.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  91. Imre I, Di Rocco RT, Brown GE, Johnson NS. Habituation of adult sea lamprey repeatedly exposed to damage-released alarm and predator cues. Environ Biol Fishes. 2016;99:613–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Smith RJF. Testosterone eliminates alarm substance in male fathead minnows. Can J Zool. 1973;51:875–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Smith RJF. Seasonal loss of alarm substance cells in north-american cyprinoid fishes and Its relation to abrasive spawning behavior. Can J Zool. 1976;54:1172–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Ferrari MCO, Trowell JJ, Brown GE, Chivers DP. The role of learning in the development of threat-sensitive predator avoidance by fathead minnows. Anim Behav. 2005;70:777–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Kelley JL, Phillips B, Cummins GH, Shand J. Changes in the visual environment affect colour signal brightness and shoaling behaviour in a freshwater fish. Anim Behav. 2012;83:783–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Kok ACM, van Hulten D, Timmerman KH, Lankhorst J, Visser F, Slabbekoorn H. Interacting effects of short-term and long-term noise exposure on antipredator behaviour in sand gobies. Anim Behav. 2021;172:93–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Bolger T, Connolly PL. The selection of suitable indexes for the measurement and analysis of fish condition. J Fish Biol. 1989;34:171–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Sargent RC. Paternal care and egg survival both increase with clutch size in the fathead minnow, Pimephales promelas. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1988;23:33–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  99. Thünken T, Meuthen D, Bakker TCM, Kullmann H. Parental investment in relation to offspring quality in the biparental cichlid fish Pelvicachromis taeniatus. Anim Behav. 2010;80:69–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Siegel AF. Robust regression using repeated medians. Biometrika. 1982;69:242–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Conradt L, Roper TJ. Activity synchrony and social cohesion: a fission-fusion model. Proc R Soc B. 2000;267:2213–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  102. Buske C, Gerlai R. Early embryonic ethanol exposure impairs shoaling and the dopaminergic and serotoninergic systems in adult zebrafish. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2011;33:698–707.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing; 2020.

  104. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;1:1–48.

    Google Scholar 

  105. Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB, Jensen SP: LmerTest: Tests in linear mixed effects models. R package version 3.1-3. 2020.

  106. Lenth RV, Buerkner P, Herve M, Love J, Riebl H, Singmann H: Emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. R package version 1.4. 2020.

  107. Torchiano M. effsize: efficient effect size computation. R package version 0.8.1. 2020.

  108. Stoffel MA, Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. partR2: partitioning R2 in generalized linear mixed models. PeerJ. 2021;9:e11414.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Adam Crane and Arash Salahinejad for helping with fish maintenance. We would also like to thank Frederik Tiedemann for his help with automating R analyses; likewise, we are grateful to Tim Schmoll and Alfredo Sánchez-Tójar for statistical discussion.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. This research was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) (ME 4974/1–1, ME 4974/2 − 1), by a scholarship from the Bielefelder Nachwuchsfonds (awarded to Denis Meuthen), and by Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) grants to Douglas P. Chivers and Maud C. O. Ferrari.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

DM, DPC, and MCOF conceived the study and designed the experiments. DM conducted the experiment, collected the data, analyzed it and wrote the manuscript. All authors improved the manuscript, read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Denis Meuthen.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The present study followed the Canadian Council on Animal Care guidelines for humane animal use and were approved by the University of Saskatchewan’s Animal Research Ethics Board (Animal Use Protocol: 20170089).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Section 1. Breeding, rearing and alarm cue exposure protocol. Section 2. Supporting results: Variation in parental care intensity. Section 3. Supporting results: Variation in the change in shoaling density. Section 4. Supporting discussion: Other factors affecting parental care intensity. Table S1. Full and final linear mixed-effect models analysing the factors explaining variation in Pimephales promelas parental care intensity, with risk treatment as the fixed effect of interest. Table S2. Full and final linear mixed-effect models analysing the factors explaining variation in average body size within shoals and within-shoal variation in body size, respectively. Table S3. Impact of average fish size within shoals and shoal heterogeneity on prestimulus shoaling density and the change in shoaling density when density is measured in a absolute distances, i.e., centimeters and b relative distances, i.e., # of body lengths. Fig. S1. Average (over a 10-min period) interindividual distances (mean ± SE) of 39-day old fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas) before (prestimulus) and after (poststimulus) a mechanical predator disturbance.

Additional file 2.

All data generated and analyzed in this study.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Meuthen, D., Ferrari, M.C.O. & Chivers, D.P. Paternal care effects outweigh gamete-mediated and personal environment effects during the transgenerational estimation of risk in fathead minnows. BMC Ecol Evo 21, 187 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-021-01919-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-021-01919-1

Keywords