Skip to main content

Experimental evolution under hyper-promiscuity in Drosophila melanogaster

Abstract

Background

The number of partners that individuals mate with over their lifetime is a defining feature of mating systems, and variation in mate number is thought to be a major driver of sexual evolution. Although previous research has investigated the evolutionary consequences of reductions in the number of mates, we know little about the costs and benefits of increased numbers of mates. Here, we use a genetic manipulation of mating frequency in Drosophila melanogaster to create a novel, highly promiscuous mating system. We generated D. melanogaster populations in which flies were deficient for the sex peptide receptor (SPR) gene – resulting in SPR- females that mated more frequently – and genetically-matched control populations, and allowed them to evolve for 55 generations. At several time-points during this experimental evolution, we assayed behavioural, morphological and transcriptional reproductive phenotypes expected to evolve in response to increased population mating frequencies.

Results

We found that males from the high mating frequency SPR- populations evolved decreased ability to inhibit the receptivity of their mates and decreased copulation duration, in line with predictions of decreased per-mating investment with increased sperm competition. Unexpectedly, SPR- population males also evolved weakly increased sex peptide (SP) gene expression. Males from SPR- populations initially (i.e., before experimental evolution) exhibited more frequent courtship and faster time until mating relative to controls, but over evolutionary time these differences diminished or reversed.

Conclusions

In response to experimentally increased mating frequency, SPR- males evolved behavioural responses consistent with decreased male post-copulatory investment at each mating and decreased overall pre-copulatory performance. The trend towards increased SP gene expression might plausibly relate to functional differences in the two domains of the SP protein. Our study highlights the utility of genetic manipulations of animal social and sexual environments coupled with experimental evolution.

Background

Mating systems in nature range from strict monogamy across a lifespan to mating with many mating partners [1]. Striking natural variation in the number of mates is often observed between closely related species and even within species (e.g., [2]). This variation has important implications for a wide array of evolutionary and ecological processes, including sexual selection, sexual conflict, social organization and evolution, life history evolution, sexually-transmitted pathogen dynamics, speciation, sperm competition and cryptic female choice [38]. As a result, there is great potential for variation in the number of mates to drive phenotypic evolution in both sexes. Because the number of mates and mating frequency are the outcome of interacting male and female mating strategies, understanding how mating systems evolve requires characterizing optimal mating strategies in both sexes and how they co-evolve between the sexes.

A powerful tool for investigating the evolutionary consequences of variation in mate numbers is experimental evolution. Several studies have adopted this approach by evolving populations at natural or restricted numbers of partners. These studies have revealed that variation in mating frequency can drive the evolution of a wide range of phenotypes, including mating behaviour [912], sperm competitiveness [9, 13], genital and gonad size [1416], cuticular hydrocarbons [17], mate harming and resistance to harm [18, 19], cognitive function [20], and sex-specific transcription [21].

However, we know little about the evolutionary consequences of increased mating frequency. This is because restricting mating is experimentally tractable, but increasing mating frequency is challenging because high mating frequencies are constrained by limited sexual receptivity in one or both sexes. To overcome this limitation, several studies have experimentally manipulated ecological settings to create conditions favouring higher mating frequencies. For example, mating frequency often increases when the sex with the highest sexual receptivity (typically males) is rarer in a population (e.g., female biased sex ratios [10, 2227]). These studies demonstrate that when the ecological setting favours increased mating, evolutionary change in mating and life history traits can occur, including increased investment in male reproductive organs [26], increased courtship frequency [28], decreased male ejaculate depletion [26, 29], increased male stimulation of female oviposition after mating [24], increased suppression of female receptivity [30], and decreased male but increased female lifespan [22, 28]. Yet, the evolutionary consequences of increases in mating frequency that are driven by changes in mating behaviour, independent of environmental variation, remain unknown.

Here, we use a genetic manipulation of the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster to experimentally increase mating frequencies in experimental populations. We allowed the populations to evolve under these hyper-promiscuous mating conditions to explore the evolutionary consequences for both sexes. To manipulate mating frequencies, we used flies deficient in the sex peptide receptor (SPR) gene [31], which encodes a female receptor for the male seminal protein, sex peptide (SP or Acp70A) [32, 33]. Sex peptide elicits several post-mating responses in females via the SPR, including inhibiting female sexual receptivity [31]. Thus, females that lack the SPR (hereafter SPR-) have a greatly reduced refractory period after mating (i.e., they become sexually receptive more quickly) [31]. When SPR is removed from populations, males are less able to prevent females from re-mating, creating a novel, highly promiscuous mating system. The increase in mating frequency in SPR- populations is within the range of naturally occurring mating frequencies found in closely related, more promiscuous drosophilid species.

We allowed SPR- and control populations to evolve for 55 generations. We first confirmed that mating frequencies were elevated in SPR- populations and tracked changes in mating frequency over evolutionary time. We then assayed behavioural, morphological and transcriptional phenotypes expected to evolve in response to increased population mating frequencies, at several time points during experimental evolution. Theory suggests that, under increased promiscuity, selection on males should shift from pre-copulatory (e.g., on courtship and mating success) to post-copulatory (e.g., on sperm competition and its avoidance), and that males should decrease per-mating ejaculate investment because the returns from each mating are likely to be lower [3439]. We therefore tested for experimentally evolved differences in both pre- and post-copulatory phenotypes in males, including courtship behaviour, copulation duration, effects on female post-mating fecundity and re-mating behaviour, testes and accessory gland size, and success in sperm competition. Because males in SPR- populations cannot influence female re-mating through SP, male investment in SP should decrease, as should male investment in the related seminal protein Ductus ejaculatorius peptide 99B (Dup99B), which also binds to SPR [31] and suppresses female post-mating receptivity (although to a lesser degree than SP [32]). To this end, we investigated the expression of both SP and Dup99B genes using RT-qPCR. We also assayed female mating strategies – including female latency to mating, post-mating fecundity and re-mating behaviour – to determine how the reproductive behaviour of the sexes co-evolves under the novel mating regime.

Results

We experimentally evolved 4 replicate SPR- populations (in which no individuals expressed SPR) and 4 genetically matched control populations, from a Dahomey stock into which the w 1118 allele for white eye colour had been backcrossed to facilitate tracking the SPR deficiency. We introduced the SPR deficiency to the stock population by backcrossing in the deletion Df(1)Exel6234 ([31]; see also [40]), which carries a white + transgene that partially rescues the w 1118 mutation. Thus, the 4 SPR- populations bore the SPR deficiency and the w + transgene and had red eyes, whereas the 4 control populations bore wild-type SPR and had white eyes. White-eyed flies have reduced vision relative to wild-type (red) eyes [41]; therefore, the red-eyed SPR- flies likely had partially improved visual performance compared with controls. For each replicate population, each generation began with 100 adult males and 100 adult females, which were permitted to interact in chambers for 9 days before progeny were collected for the next generation. Initial tests of male courtship behaviour revealed that SPR- and white-eyed control males had comparable courtship rates, whereas wild-type males courted females significantly more (>50 % more frequently) than both SPR- and white-eyed males (see Additional file 1: Supporting results). Due to this substantial difference in courtship behaviour we therefore did not include wild-type populations in the subsequent experimental evolution.

For most phenotypes, we first used unselected SPR- and control flies to establish baseline differences caused by the SPR deficiency itself or by the difference in eye colour between control and SPR- flies. Thus, ‘unselected SPR-’ and ‘unselected control’ refer to flies that have not undergone experimental evolution, and for which any differences are a result of the genetic manipulation rather than evolution under varying levels of promiscuity. We then tested for evolved differences between the replicate SPR- and control populations.

Behavioural evolution

For most behavioural phenotypes, we first tested for effects of the SPR deficiency itself in both sexes. We then tested for evolved differences in pairings between (1) experimentally-evolved males and wild-type females, (2) experimentally-evolved females and wild-type males, and (3) experimentally-evolved males and females within treatments (Table 1).

Table 1 Summary of tests for phenotypic differences related to experimental evolution under high promiscuity

Mating frequency

As expected, we observed mating ~15-fold more frequently in populations of SPR- males and SPR- females, compared with control populations (Fig. 1a). This difference was maintained during experimental evolution despite overall declining mating frequency (Fig. 1b).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mating frequency over the course of experimental evolution. (a) The percentage of time in which flies were observed mating in replicate populations (1–4) in which flies did (Control) or did not express sex peptide receptor (SPR-). (b) The proportion of time in copula for SPR- populations relative to control populations

Courtship frequency

Unselected SPR- males (i.e., males lacking SPR, but not experimentally evolved) courted wild-type females more frequently than did unselected control males (Table 2), likely due to improved vision in SPR- populations relative to controls. At generation 16, experimentally-evolved SPR- males similarly courted females more frequently than control males, although the difference was not statistically significant (P = 0.06). However, at generation 26 we detected no difference nor any trend for experimentally-evolved SPR- males to court more frequently (Table 2), suggesting an evolved loss of courtship activity in the hyper-promiscuous SPR- lines relative to control lines.

Table 2 Male courtship frequency

Latency until mating

Unselected SPR- males were faster to mate with wild-type females, at both a male’s first mating and fifth consecutive mating (Table 3), as expected from the relatively better vision of SPR- males compared with controls. Although experimentally-evolved SPR- males were also faster to achieve first matings at generation 16, we detected no difference at generation 36 nor any trend for SPR- males to mate faster. At generation 36, the pattern had reversed, with experimentally-evolved control males being faster than SPR- males to achieve a fifth mating (Table 3). This suggests that SPR- males lost their mating speed advantage (relative to control males) over evolutionary time.

Table 3 Latency until mating

We found no evidence for evolutionary change in latency to mating in either SPR- females or in within-treatment pairings. SPR- females were faster to mate with wild-type males than control females in both unselected (albeit not significantly so) and experimentally-evolved flies, with a similar magnitude of difference (Table 3). Likewise, SPR- males and females paired together were faster to mate than controls in both unselected and experimentally evolved flies (Table 3).

Copulation duration

The SPR deficiency itself did not alter male copulation duration in unselected flies, but experimentally-evolved SPR- males had shorter copulations than controls (Fig. 2; Table 4). In contrast, the SPR deficiency itself caused a shorter female copulation duration, but experimentally-evolved SPR- and control females did not differ (Fig. 2; Table 4). In within-treatment pairings, we found only weak evidence that the SPR deficiency itself affected copulation duration (Fig. 2; Table 4). However, experimentally-evolved SPR- males and females had significantly shorter matings (Fig. 2, Table 4). The above results are for matings between virgin flies; we found no differences in copulation duration in second matings (Additional file 1: Table S2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Copulation duration in unselected and experimentally evolved SPR- and control flies. Copulation duration measured in flies that either did (control, C) or did not express sex peptide receptor (SPR-), for unselected flies (in which any differences between C and SPR- flies are caused by the SPR deficiency itself) and experimentally-evolved selected flies. Dotted lines demarcate different comparisons where we tested for experimentally evolved differences in males by pairing SPR- or C males with wild-type females (at generation 16 or 36), in females by pairing SPR- or C females with wild-type males (at generation 26), or in within-treatment pairings of SPR- (or C) males with SPR- (or C) females (measured at generation 26). Asterisks indicate significant differences between SPR- and C flies. Least squares means are presented with 95 % confidence intervals

Table 4 Copulation duration

Post-mating fecundity

Neither unselected nor experimentally-evolved SPR- and control males differed in their ability to stimulate female oviposition (Additional file 1: Table S3). When expressed in females, the SPR deficiency itself reduced fecundity in unselected flies, as expected given SP’s role in fecundity stimulation [42] (Additional file 1: Table S3). Although we hypothesized that experimentally evolved SPR- females would evolve relatively increased post-mating fecundity, through selection to decrease reliance on SP, the difference in egg-laying at generation 26 was similar to that caused by the SPR deficiency in unselected flies (Additional file 1: Table S3).

The SPR- deficiency did not alter male fertility (proportion of eggs developing into adults: median and range, SPR-, 0.80 [0.29, 0.98]; control, 0.80 [0.00, 0.92]; Wilcoxon χ2 1 = 0.2, P = 0.66), suggesting that fertility differences did not confound the sperm competition experiments (results below).

Re-mating behaviour

As expected given that SPR is not known to impact male ejaculate parameters, the SPR deficiency did not affect male ability to inhibit re-mating by their wild-type mates, whether measured as latency to re-mating (Table 5) or (in a separate experiment) as the number of females re-mating (generation 0: initial mating with SPR- male: 11/20; initial mating with control male: 9/18; χ2 1 = 0.1, P = 0.76). Although experimentally-evolved males likewise did not differ in ability to inhibit re-mating at generation 16, SPR- males were significantly worse at inhibiting re-mating at generation 36 (Table 5).

Table 5 Re-mating behaviour

Surprisingly – given that SP inhibits female re-mating – when the SPR- deficiency was expressed in unselected females, we did not detect significant changes in latency until re-mating (Table 5); nonetheless, the confidence interval for the difference between groups is 0.9 – 2.3 (where 1 denotes identical risk ratios), indicating no strong support for rejecting the null hypothesis of no difference. However, experimentally-evolved SPR- females re-mated more quickly after mating with wild-type males (Table 5). In within-treatment pairings, we found the expected faster re-mating following matings between SPR- males and SPR- females, compared with control males and control females, with a similar magnitude of difference for both unselected and experimentally-evolved flies (Table 5).

Morphological evolution

Evolving in populations that lacked SPR had no significant effect on testes or accessory gland size – neither on absolute organ size nor on body mass-corrected size – or on body mass, nor did the SPR deficiency itself influence these traits (Additional file 1: Table S5). We tested whether the SPR deficiency influenced female mass as a test for pleiotropic effects of the deficiency, but found no difference (Additional file 1: Table S5).

Ejaculate evolution

Sperm precedence as first male to mate

Neither the SPR deficiency itself nor evolution in SPR- populations affected either male success in sperm competition as the first male to mate (P1) or male ability to maintain P1 over multiple matings (Table 6).

Table 6 Sperm precedence

Sperm precedence as second male to mate

We found mixed evidence that the SPR deficiency itself affected male success in sperm competition as the second male to mate (P2), but experimentally evolved SPR- and control males showed little difference (Table 6).

Sex peptide and Dup99B gene expression evolution

Surprisingly, we found a trend towards higher SP expression in experimentally evolved SPR- males, compared with controls (F1,6 = 5.1, P = 0.06; Fig. 3a). In contrast, there was no significant difference in Dup99B expression (F1,6 = 0.5, P = 0.49; Fig. 3b). Characteristics of the RT-qPCR calibration curves are given in Additional file 1 (Table S8).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Experimental evolution of sex peptide (SP) and Dup99B gene expression. Mean levels of gene expression of (a) SP and (b) Dup99B in four experimentally evolved replicate populations each of the sex peptide receptor-deficient (SPR-) treatment (in which neither sex expressed SPR) and the control treatment. Gene expression was measured at generation 55. Error bars represent ± 2 S.E

Discussion

Our study investigated the evolutionary consequences of increased mating frequencies above naturally occurring levels. Our main findings are that, after evolution at high mating frequency in SPR- populations, males from these populations evolved shorter copulation duration and decreased ability to inhibit female re-mating. These changes are consistent with decreased male post-copulatory investment at each mating. Surprisingly, males from SPR- populations also tended to show an evolved increase in SP gene expression, and we discuss hypotheses that might explain this pattern below. Furthermore, although SPR- males initially courted females more frequently and mated faster than controls, we found that after experimental evolution these differences were no longer detectable (for courtship frequency and time until first mating) or had reversed (for time until fifth mating), results consistent with decreased pre-copulatory investment.

Reduced ejaculate investment

Sperm competition theory predicts that males should decrease their investment in ejaculate at each mating as the intensity of sperm competition (i.e., the number of competing ejaculates from different males) increases because the returns from investing in a given mating decrease [3438]. We confirmed that mating frequency was increased in SPR- populations, such that sperm competition would have been intensified if increased mating frequency resulted in greater temporal overlap of ejaculates from multiple males (e.g., [43]) . This is an especially likely scenario here because SPR regulates female sperm storage, such that SPR- females retain more sperm in the days after mating [44].

Two lines of evidence support the prediction that per-mating ejaculate investment decreased in males from SPR- populations. First, SPR- males evolved decreased copulation duration compared with control males. Copulation duration in matings with virgin females is positively correlated with ejaculate transfer in Drosophila spp. ([45, 46]; but see [47]), suggesting that SPR- males might transfer less ejaculate per mating. Second, SPR- males were less able to inhibit female re-mating, consistent with the hypothesis that they transfer fewer receptivity-inhibiting seminal proteins. Below, we discuss hypotheses to reconcile this result with our finding of increased SP gene expression for these males. Although these results are consistent with decreased per-mating ejaculate investment, we did not find differences in either male stimulation of female fecundity or success in sperm competition. Low statistical power is a potential concern, but our sample sizes were sufficient to allow detection of some effects of the SPR deficiency itself on these traits. Taken together, these changes in ejaculate-related traits are consistent with decreased investment in some ejaculate components more than others and a shift in ejaculate composition [48].

Expression of sex peptide

If expressing SP is costly, then males co-evolving with SPR- females should evolve lower SP gene expression because SP is functionless with these females. There are several potential explanations for our finding that SP expression was weakly increased, in addition to the possibility that the marginally non-significant result (P = 0.06) arose by chance. First, expressing SP may not be costly for males; however, this alone does not explain increased SP expression in SPR- males. Second, SP expression measured in males might not reflect SP protein transfer to females at mating. Indeed, a recent study found no correlation between the two [49]. SP expression in males could instead reflect male SP stores, with larger stores evolving to increase male capacity to mate many times in SPR- populations. However, we detected no change in accessory gland size, nor evidence that SPR- males could better sustain ejaculate transfer over multiple matings, as SPR- and control males had similar success in sperm competition in first and fifth matings.

Third, if SP expression is in fact positively correlated with SP protein transfer, then increased SP expression in SPR- males appears inconsistent with our finding of decreased ability of SPR- males to inhibit female re-mating. However, if functionality of the SP protein degraded through genetic drift, a likely scenario given that SP is effectively neutral in SPR- lines, this inconsistency is reconciled. Under this scenario, there are two potential explanations for increased SP gene expression. First, SP expression might be genetically correlated with other ejaculate components under positive selection (e.g., to compensate for SP in SPR- populations). A second mechanism relates to SP protein structure, which consists of a carboxy-terminal end that binds to SPR and mediates female post-mating oviposition and re-mating responses, and an amino-terminal end that does not bind to SPR but to unknown female receptors [31]. Positive selection on the amino-terminal end might occur in SPR- populations to compensate for the lack of oviposition stimulation from the carboxy-terminal end, because the amino-terminal end induces juvenile hormone synthesis in adult females and stimulates oogenesis ([50, 51]; see also [52]). We did not find that SPR- males increased the post-mating fecundity of wild-type females, although the wild-type female fecundity response might be dominated by carboxy-terminal end effects. This hypothesis is consistent with our finding of no increase in Dup99B expression, as Dup99B is homologous with SP only at the carboxy-terminal end. Finally, SP might have other as yet unknown functions that are selected positively under high promiscuity.

Evidence for decreased pre-copulatory performance

We found two effects on pre-copulatory performance that were most likely caused by the improved vision of red-eyed SPR- males relative to white-eyed controls: unselected SPR- males courted females more frequently and were faster to mate. Both effects remained detectable at generation 16, but by generation 36, both were either not statistically significant or significant and reversed (with control males faster to achieve a fifth mating). Although type II error is a possibility such that we simply failed to detect the same differences, the trends were no longer in the previous direction: at generation 36, mean courtship frequency was higher in control males and the risk ratio for latency to mating was 1.0. The results are consistent with decreased pre-copulatory investment in SPR- males, which might result from weaker selection to overcome re-mating resistance with SPR- females. Declining courtship frequency might be accompanied by decreased courtship intensity or quality of courtship signals. Furthermore, if the increased mating frequency in SPR- populations caused sexual selection to shift from pre-copulatory to post-copulatory, we would expect increased displays of post-mating guarding behaviour (recently reported in D. melanogaster [53]) in SPR- males. We are currently investigating these hypotheses.

Conclusions

Our study shows that SPR- males evolving at high promiscuity evolve shorter copulations and decreased inhibition of female re-mating, consistent with decreased post-copulatory investment per mating, and less frequent courtship and longer latency until mating, consistent with decreased pre-copulatory investment. Curiously, males that evolved with SPR- females also showed a tendency for increased SP expression. Previous experimental evolution studies have demonstrated that reducing a population’s mating frequency by imposing monogamy can result in evolutionary change in reproductive traits (e.g., [10, 23, 54]). Other studies have increased or decreased mating frequency by altering the sex ratio (e.g., [2426, 28]), but changes in sex ratio tend to alter mating frequency and mate competition in opposite directions (e.g., with fewer matings but more intense competition at male-biased sex ratios). The effects of increased promiscuity per se, as observed in our study, seem to be different from simultaneously changing mating frequency and mate competition. Our study highlights the utility of genetic manipulations of the socio-sexual environment, and the sensitivity of experimental evolution studies of mating system to the method by which mating frequency is manipulated.

Methods

Fly stocks and experimental populations

All flies shared an outbred, lab-adapted Dahomey genetic background. To set up the control and experimental populations, we first generated a white-eyed Dahomey background population by backcrossing a loss of function allele (w 1118) for the X-linked white gene into the Dahomey background, yielding white eyes [55]. We then derived the SPR- populations by backcrossing the deletion mutation Df(1)Exel6234 ([31]; see also [40]), which covers the entire X-linked SPR gene and four linked loci, into the white-eyed Dahomey background population for 5 generations (Additional file 2: Figure S1). The mutation carries a white+ transgene, which provides a partial rescue of the w 1118 mutation. Thus, in a w 1118 background, male hemizygote and female homozygote carriers of Df(1)Exel6234 possess red eyes, whereas heterozygote females possess orange eyes, which facilitates tracking the SPR deficiency.

We used these populations to establish 4 replicate SPR- populations that did not express SPR and 4 genetically-matched control populations, for a total of 8 populations [40] (Additional file 2: Figure S1). Replicate populations were maintained for 36 generations as follows. Each generation began with 100 adult males and 100 adult females permitted to interact in 4.5 L chambers for 9 days. Each chamber contained three bottles of food media (50 mL) with granules of live yeast. Bottles were replaced on days 4 and 7. After 9 days, eggs were collected for propagation of the next generation and transferred at standardized density to bottles. Adults were collected 13 days later. From generations 36–45, populations were maintained by transferring adults to new food every two weeks at an uncontrolled sex ratio and density. From generations 0–45, flies were maintained and experiments conducted on sugar-yeast-molasses medium in plastic bottles or vials at 25 °C on a 12:12 h light:dark cycle.

At generation 45, each replicate population was divided among 10 vials with Jazz-mix food medium (Fisher Scientific), with 3 males and 3 females per vial. Virgin progeny were collected and housed in sex-specific vials. For each population, progeny from all 10 vials were pooled and 30 males and 30 females were used to start the next generation. Three virgin males and 3 virgin females were housed per vial for a two-day oviposition period, after which they were removed and offspring were allowed to develop for 10–11 days.

We conducted two further rounds of backcrossing Df(1)Exel6234 into the white-eyed Dahomey background population to generate unselected SPR- and control flies that had not undergone experimental evolution (Additional file 2: Figure S1), in order to further test for differences caused by the deficiency itself at generations 26 and 36 (in addition to initial tests at generation 0).

To conduct the sperm competition experiments (at generation 36), we established additional populations to aid in distinguishing evolved responses from effects of the Df(1)Exel6234 deficiency itself. These populations consisted of (1) four ‘SPR+ evolved control’ populations, in which the genetic background was that of populations that evolved with females expressing SPR, but through backcrossing they carried Df(1)Exel6234 and had red eyes; or, conversely, (2) four ‘SPR- evolved control’ populations, in which the genetic background was that of populations that evolved without SPR expression, but through backcrossing they carried wild-type SPR and had white eyes (Additional file 2: Figures S2 and S3). We refer to these ‘SPR- evolved control’ and ‘SPR+ evolved control’ populations together as ‘experimentally-evolved backcrossed controls’. To generate the four SPR- evolved control populations, beginning at generation 26, we backcrossed Df(1)Exel6234 into the four control populations (i.e., the four control populations as described above, having undergone 26 generations of experimental evolution under controlled conditions). We did this by crossing virgin females from each control population with males from an unselected SPR- population. Heterozygote orange-eyed virgin daughters were backcrossed with males from each respective (parental) control population. After 6 generations of backcrossing, orange-eyed daughters were crossed with red eyed males from the same replicate for 4 generations to generate red eyed progeny (Additional file 2: Figure S2). We conducted analogous backcrossing to generate the four SPR+ evolved control populations (Additional file 2: Figure S3).

In the sperm competition experiments, we used competitor males that were homozygous for the recessive sparkling (spa) mutation, which causes a rough eye phenotype in homozygotes, backcrossed into a Dahomey background.

Tests of experimentally evolving phenotypes

We conducted five sets of experiments (Table 1): (1) initial tests using unselected flies that had not undergone experimental evolution (i.e., immediately after backcrossing, at generations 0, 26 and 36 as described above), to establish baseline differences and test for effects of the Df(1)Exel6234 deficiency itself (a necessary step, as differences between control and SPR- flies might occur due to the difference in eye colour used a phenotypic marker for the deletion, pleiotropic effects of SPR in either sex, or effects of the four genes linked to SPR- in the Df(1)Exel6234 deficiency; 31); (2) at generation 16, tests of evolved male phenotypes, by pairing experimentally evolved males with standard wild-type females which are described below; (3) at generation 26, tests of evolved female phenotypes, by pairing experimentally evolved females with standard wild-type males, and tests of co-evolved male and female traits, by pairing male and females of the same treatment; (4) at generation 36, tests of sperm competitiveness; and (5) at generation 55, tests of the evolution of SP and Dup99B gene expression. Standard wild-type males and females were derived from the Dahomey stock population maintained in our laboratory in population cages housing thousands of flies. All flies used in the behavioural, morphological and sperm competition experiments, including standard wild-type and experimental flies, were reared at standard density by allowing females to oviposit on petri dishes containing agar-grape juice medium, from which first instar larvae were transferred to bottles containing standard food, at a density of 100 larvae/7.5 mL food. Flies were collected as virgins within six hours of eclosion using ice anesthesia, housed in same-sex vials containing food medium and live yeast in groups of 15–20 for males and 10–15 for females, transferred to fresh vials every 2–3 days, and were 2–5 days post-emergence when used in experiments.

Behavioural evolution

Except where stated, we transferred males to individual vials containing food medium and live yeast approximately 20 h before experiments began, and added females to male vials to begin each experiment. We excluded individuals for which copulation duration was less than five minutes because it is unclear whether these represent real copulations involving ejaculate transfer (e.g., [56, 57]).

Mating frequency

In 18 generations between generations 1 and 28 (17 of the first 19 generations, plus generation 28), we conducted spot check observations of male–female interactions for each replicate population (Table 1). Most observations were taken on days 2–5 of the interaction period. Over the 18 generations a total of 141 observations were taken and 2716 matings were observed.

Courtship frequency

We tested for differences in courtship frequency caused by either the SPR deficiency itself (tested in two blocks) or by experimental evolution (Table 1), by placing 5 Dahomey wild-type virgin females with five virgin males of either SPR- or control type in a vial containing food media and live yeast. We assayed multiple replicate vials in each trial, such that vial was the unit of replication (for effects of the SPR deficiency: N = 17–20 per group in block 1, N = 31 per group in block 2; for experimentally evolved differences: N = 10 per group). We conducted spot check observations several times daily over four days, in which we recorded the number of courtship events per vial.

Latency until mating

We tested for differences in the latency until mating in virgin flies of both sexes, caused by either the SPR deficiency itself or experimental evolution (Table 1), by pairing SPR- or control males (or females) with virgin wild-type Dahomey females (or males). We also tested for differences in within-population pairings of experimentally evolved males and females, in which SPR- males and females were paired together and control males and females were paired together. We also measured latency to mating as part of the sperm competition experiments (see below).

Copulation duration

We measured copulation duration for a first mating between virgin flies in the experiments conducted to measure latency until mating described above (Table 1). We also measured copulation duration in a second mating in the experiments involving SPR- and control females paired with wild-type males, and for within-treatment pairings (described above), by pairing females with a new male after their initial mating. We also measured copulation duration for virgin and previously-mated females as part of the sperm competition experiments (see below).

Post-mating fecundity and fertility

We tested for differences in the ability of male SPR- and control males to stimulate egg-laying by wild-type Dahomey females, and for differences in egg-laying by SPR- and control females following mating with a wild-type male (Table 1). In both experiments, we counted the number of eggs females laid in individual vials containing food medium in the 24 h following a single mating. At generation 0, we also tested for differences in male ability to fertilize eggs caused by the deficiency alone. We measured fertility as the proportion of eggs developing into adults, by counting adult offspring 12 days after females were removed from vials. We measured egg fertility to ensure that differences in fertility were not a confounding factor in the sperm competition experiments.

Latency until re-mating

We tested for differences in latency to re-mating for (1) SPR- and control male ability to inhibit re-mating by wild-type Dahomey mates, (2) SPR- and control female propensity to re-mate with wild-type Dahomey males, and (3) within-population pairings. We measured these responses as part of the experiments described above (see ‘Latency until mating’).

To measure latency to re-mating in females after matings with SPR- or control males, we separated females from males following a single mating. Females were housed for 24 h in individual vials and allowed to oviposit so that we could measure egg production. After 24 h, we transferred females to a new vial containing food medium and a virgin wild-type Dahomey male. We recorded the time of transfer and the time mating began. We followed a similar procedure to measure SPR- and control female propensity to re-mate with a wild-type male, following an initial mating with a wild-type male. We also followed a similar procedure in the within-population experiment to measure SPR- and control female propensity to re-mate with a wild-type male, following an initial mating with a male of the same replicate population as the female. We also measured latency to re-mating as part of the sperm competition experiments (see below).

Morphological evolution

We tested for differences in male mass and male testes and accessory gland size that were caused by the deficiency itself (in unselected flies) or the result of experimental evolution (Table 1). We also measured female mass in unselected flies to test for pleiotropic effects of the SPR deficiency. To measure male and female mass, we weighed virgin flies to the nearest 10−3 mg. To measure testes and accessory gland size, we dissected testes and accessory glands from virgin males in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS), mounted them individually on microscope slides, photographed them along with a one-millimeter measure, and calculated their area from the digital photographs using ImageJ software [58]. We measured both testes and calculated the mean area per testis, and likewise calculated mean area per accessory gland.

Ejaculate evolution

Sperm competitiveness

We tested male sperm competitiveness when males were first or second to mate with a female (i.e., in the disfavoured P1 and favoured P2 roles, respectively) in two separate experiments. In the P1 experiment, we tested male sperm competitiveness when males mated for the first time or fifth consecutive time, to test male responses when ejaculate stores were full or depleted, respectively. We tested unselected SPR- and control males that had not undergone experimental evolution to test for effects of the Df(1)Exel6234 deficiency itself, as well as experimentally evolved SPR- and control males, and males from the experimentally-evolved backcrossed control populations described above..

In each experiment, focal males competed against competitor males that were homozygous for the recessive sparkling (spa) mutation, which causes a rough eye phenotype in homozygotes. Females in the sperm competition experiments were also homozygous for spa, thus allowing us to identify offspring fathered by the experimental or competitor males by eye phenotype [59]. In the P1 experiment, focal males were mated with females on day one of the experiment. Females were then transferred individually to new vials and spa competitors were introduced for re-mating on the following day. When females did not re-mate on the second day, the spa male was removed and a new spa male was introduced on the third day. We repeated the experiment to constitute two blocks. In the P2 experiment, we followed a similar procedure, with spa males mated with females on day one of the experiment and focal males introduced for re-mating on the following day. We conducted a single replicate of this experiment. In both experiments, we recorded latency to mating and copulation duration.

After an initial mating, the first and 5th females to mate with a male were transferred individually to separate new vials containing food media and yeast granules and allowed to oviposit for 24 h. We scored adult offspring 12 days later.

Sex peptide and Dup99B gene expression

To prepare flies for measurement of gene expression, at generation 55 we standardized larval density within each replicate population by pairing 3–4 day old virgin males and females for 24 h, with one pair per vial in each of 15 vials. Males were then removed and females permitted to oviposit on agar-grape juice plates. Eggs were transferred to vials containing food medium (40 eggs per vial, 5 vials per replicate population). Emerging male offspring were collected as virgins and housed individually.

Four virgin males, each 6 days old, from each replicate population were dissected and their abdomens stored individually in RNAlater reagent (Sigma-Aldrich) at 4 °C for 24 h and then at −80 °C. Prior to RNA extraction, the abdominal tissue was frozen using liquid nitrogen and homogenized. Total RNA was extracted using a Purelink RNA mini kit (Ambion) using ethanol and 2-mercaptoethanol and an on-column DNase treatment (Purelink DNase I). RNA was eluted with RNA storage solution (Ambion) and stored at −80 ° C. RNA yield (quantified with Qubit 2.0 Fluorometer, Invitrogen) ranged between 3–6 μg/ml.

RT-qPCR assays were set up manually using Brilliant III Ultra-Fast SYBR Green QRT-PCR Master Mix (Stratagene, Agilent Technologies) on an Applied Biosystems 7500 Fast Real-Time PCR system. Amplification reactions were performed in 20 μl total volume with 2 μl of RNA and 0.1 μM of each primer, under the following conditions: incubation at 50 °C for 2 min, 95 °C for 10 min, followed by 40 cycles of 95 °C for 15 s and 60 °C for 1 min.

We used three technical replicates for each of the 4 biological replicate samples per population. RpL32 was the reference gene. Relative standard curves were generated with serial dilutions of RNA (1, 1/5, 1/25, 1/125, 1/625). Stock RNA for the relative standard curves was extracted from pooled whole males from the four control populations. For the calibrator sample, RNA from abdominal tissues of 15 males from each of the four control populations was pooled and diluted 10 times. Triplicate reactions of the calibrator, no-template control and no-reverse transcriptase control were used on each PCR plate. Primers for SP, Dup99B, and RpL32 were designed using NCBI Primer-BLAST (Additional file 1: Table S9) and manufactured by Genomed, Poland. The primers covered exon–intron boundaries (i.e., each primer had sequences from two exons).

Raw data were obtained from the Sequence Detection Systems Software v1.3 as mean values and standard deviations across technical replicates of the target and reference genes. Raw data were normalized using the relative standard curve method. For each biological replicate and for the calibrator sample on each plate, the mean quantity of SP or Dup99B was normalized to the mean quantity of RpL32. The fold difference between treatment sample and calibrator was calculated as the normalized value of the sample divided by the normalized value of the calibrator.

Statistical analyses

We tested for differences in courtship, copulation duration, post-mating fecundity, testes and accessory gland area, and body mass-corrected testes and accessory gland area that were related to the deficiency itself, in unselected flies, using linear models in which the fixed factor treatment had the levels SPR- and control. We tested for differences in these phenotypes in experimentally evolved flies using similar mixed models with the additional random factor of replicate population nested within treatment. We transformed responses where necessary to meet the assumptions of parametric statistics, and used a Wilcoxon test if no transformation was sufficient. Least squares means are presented with standard errors unless otherwise noted. We tested for differences in latency until mating and re-mating using proportional hazards survival models including the factor treatment (for unselected flies) or treatment and replicate population nested in treatment as a random factor (for experimentally evolved flies). Linear and survival models were performed in JMP v. 11.2.0 (SAS Institute). To analyze male success in sperm competition, we used generalized linear models specifying a quasibinomial link function, implemented in R v3.02 [60] using the MASS package [61]. Models for paternity in the P1 experiment included the factors treatment, presence of the SPR deficiency (because experiments included the additional experimentally-evolved backcrossed controls described above), the day on which re-mating occurred (i.e., the 2nd or 3rd day of the experiment), copulation durations in the first and second matings, block, and replicate population nested within treatment. Models for paternity in the P2 experiment included the factors treatment, presence of the SPR deficiency, copulation durations in the first and second mating, and replicate population nested within treatment. To examine the effect of the experimental evolution treatment on expression levels of SP and Dup99B in males from SPR- and control populations, we used mixed models as above, with the fixed factor treatment and the random factor replicate population nested within treatment. Fold difference values were log-transformed to meet the assumptions of the linear model.

Abbreviations

P1, Proportion of offspring sired by the first of two males to mate with a female; P2, Proportion of offspring sired by the second of two males to mate with a female; RT-qPCR, Reverse transcription quantitative polymerase chain reaction; SP, sex peptide; SPR, sex peptide receptor

References

  1. Andersson M. Sexual Selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Taylor ML, Price TAR, Wedell N. Polyandry in nature: a global analysis. Trends Ecol Evol. 2014;29:376–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Gavrilets S, Hayashi TI. Speciation and sexual conflict. Evol Ecol. 2005;19:167–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Wedell N, Kvarnemo C, Lessells CM, Tregenza T. Sexual conflict and life histories. Anim Behav. 2006;71:999–1011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Cornwallis CK, West SA, Davis KE, Griffin AS. Promiscuity and the evolutionary transition to complex societies. Nature. 2010;466:969–72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Simmons LW. Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Parker GA. Sexual conflict over mating and fertilization: an overview. Phil Trans Roy Soc. 2006;361:235–59.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Thrall PH, Antonovics J, Dobson AP. Sexually transmitted diseases in polygynous mating systems: prevalence and impact on reproductive success. Proc Roy Soc B. 2000;267:1555–63.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Klemme I, Firman RC. Male house mice that have evolved with sperm competition have increased mating duration and paternity success. Anim Behav. 2013;85:751–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Crudgington HS, Fellows S, Badcock NS, Snook RR. Experimental manipulation of sexual selection promotes greater male mating capacity but does not alter sperm investment. Evolution. 2009;63:926–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Martin OY, Hosken DJ. The evolution of reproductive isolation through sexual conflict. Nature. 2003;423:979–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Pitnick S, Brown WD, Miller GT. Evolution of female remating behaviour following experimental removal of sexual selection. Proc R Soc B. 2001;268:557–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Hosken DJ, Garner TWJ, Ward PI. Sexual conflict selects for male and female reproductive characters. Curr Biol. 2001;11:489–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Hosken DJ, Ward PI. Experimental evidence for testis size evolution via sperm competition. Ecol Lett. 2001;4:10–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. House CM, Lewis Z, Hodgson DJ, Wedell N, Sharma MD, Hunt J, Hosken DJ. Sexual and natural selection both influence male genital evolution. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e63807.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Pitnick S, Miller GT, Reagan J, Holland B. Males' evolutionary responses to experimental removal of sexual selection. Proc Roy Soc B. 2001;268:1071–80.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Sharma MD, Hunt J, Hosken DJ. Antagonistic responses to natural and sexual selection and the sex-specific evolution of cuticular hydrocarbons in Drosophila simulans. Evolution. 2012;66:665–77.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Martin OY, Hosken DJ. Costs and benefits of evolving under experimentally enforced polyandry or monogamy. Evolution. 2003;57(12):2765–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Gay L, Hosken DJ, Eady P, Vasudev R, Tregenza T. The evolution of harm - effect of sexual conflicts and population size. Evolution. 2011;65:725–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Hollis B, Kawecki TJ. Male cognitive performance declines in the absence of sexual selection. Proc R Soc B. 2014;281:20132873.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Hollis B, Houle D, Yan Z, Kawecki TJ, Keller L. Evolution under monogamy feminizes gene expression in Drosophila melanogaster. Nat Commun. 2014;5:3482.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Wigby S, Chapman TW. Female resistance to male harm evolves in response to manipulation of sexual conflict. Evolution. 2004;58:1028–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Crudgington HS, Beckerman AP, Brüstle L, Green K, Snook RR. Experimental removal and elevation of sexual selection: does sexual selection generate manipulative males and resistant females? Am Nat. 2005;165:S72–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Linklater JR, Wertheim B, Wigby S, Chapman T. Ejaculate depletion patterns evolve in response to experimental manipulation of sex ratio in Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution. 2007;61:2027–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Edward DA, Fricke C, Chapman T. Adaptations to sexual selection and sexual conflict: insights from experimental evolution and artificial selection. Phil Trans Roy Soc. 2010;365:2541–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Reuter M, Linklater JR, Lehmann L, Chapman T, Fowler K, Hurst GDD. Adaptation to experimental alterations of the operational sex ratio in populations of Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution. 2008;62:401–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Bacigalupe L, Crudgington HS, Slate J, Moore AJ, Snook RR. Sexual selection and interacting phenotypes in experimental evolution: a study of Drosophila pseudoobscura mating behavior. Evolution. 2008;62:1804–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Nandy B, Gupta V, Sen S, Udaykumar N, Samant MA, Ali SZ, Prasad NG. Evolution of mate-harm, longevity and behaviour in male fruit flies subjected to different levels of interlocus conflict. BMC Evol Biol. 2013;13(1):212.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Ingleby FC, Lewis Z, Wedell N. Level of sperm competition promotes evolution of male ejaculate allocation patterns in a moth. Anim Behav. 2010;80:37–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Price TAR, Lewis Z, Smith DT, Hurst GDD, Wedell N. Sex ratio drive promotes sexual conflict and sexual coevolution in the fly Drosophila pseudoobscura. Evolution. 2009;64:1504–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Yapici N, Kim Y-J, Ribeiro C, Dickson BJ. A receptor that mediates the post-mating switch in Drosophila reproductive behaviour. Nature. 2008;451:33–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Liu H, Kubli E. Sex-peptide is the molecular basis of the sperm effect in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;100:9929–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Chapman T, Bangham J, Vinti G, Seifried B, Lung O, Wolfner MF, Smith HK, Partridge L. The sex peptide of Drosophila melanogaster: female post-mating responses analyzed by using RNA interference. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100(17):9923–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Ball MA, Parker GA. Sperm competition games: inter-and intra-species results of a continuous external fertilization model. J Theor Biol. 1997;186:459–66.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Parker GA, Ball MA, Stockley P, Gage MJG. Sperm competition games: individual assessment of sperm competition intensity by group spawners. Proc R Soc B. 1996;263:1291–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Williams PD, Day T, Cameron E. The evolution of sperm-allocation strategies and the degree of sperm competition. Evolution. 2005;59:492–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Parker GA, Lessells CM, Simmons LW. Sperm competition games: a general model for precopulatory male–male competition. Evolution. 2013;67:95–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Parker GA, Ball MA. Sperm competition, mating rate and the evolution of testis and ejaculate sizes: a population model. Biol Lett. 2005;1:235–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Parker GA. Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Biol Rev. 1970;45:525–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Dean R, Perry JC, Pizzari T, Mank JE, Wigby S. Experimental evolution of a novel sexually antagonistic allele. PLoS Genet. 2012;8:e1002917.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Geer BW, Green MM. Genotype, phenotype and mating behavior of Drosophila melanogaster. Am Nat. 1962;96:175–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Kubli E. Sex-peptides: seminal peptides of the Drosophila male. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2003;60:1689–704.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Turnell BR, Shaw KL. High opportunity for postcopulatory sexual selection under field conditions. Evolution. 2015;69(8):2094–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Avila FW, Mattei AL, Wolfner MF. Sex peptide receptor is required for the release of stored sperm by mated Drosophila melanogaster females. J Insect Physiol. 2015;76:1–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Mazzi D, Kesäniemi J, Hoikkala A, Klappert K. Sexual conflict over the duration of copulation in Drosophila montana: why is longer better? BMC Evol Biol. 2009;9:132.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Wigby S, Sirot LK, Linklater JR, Buehner N, Calboli FCF, Bretman A, Wolfner MF, Chapman T. Seminal fluid protein allocation and male reproductive success. Curr Biol. 2009;19:751–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Sirot L, Wolfner MF, Wigby S. Protein-specific manipulation of ejaculate composition in response to female mating status in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108:9922–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Perry JC, Sirot L, Wigby S. The seminal symphony: how to compose an ejaculate. Trends Ecol Evol. 2013;28:414–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Smith DT, Sirot LK, Wolfner MF, Hosken DJ, Wedell N. The consequences of genetic variation in sex peptide expression levels for egg laying and retention in females. Heredity. 2012;109:222–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Yamamoto R, Bai H, Dolezal AG, Amdam G, Tatar M. Juvenile hormone regulation of Drosophila aging. BMC Biol. 2013;11:85.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Reiff T, Jacobson J, Cognigni P, Antonello Z, Ballesta E, Tan KJ, Yew JY, Dominguez M, Miguel-Aliaga I. Endocrine remodelling of the adult intestine sustains reproduction in Drosophila. eLife. 2015;4:e06930.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Bontonou G, Shaik HA, Denis B, Wicker-Thomas C. Acp70A regulates Drosophila pheromones through juvenile hormone induction. Insect Biochem Mol Biol. 2015;56:36–49.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Baxter CM, Barnett R, Dukas R. Aggression, mate guarding and fitness in male fruit flies. Anim Behav. 2015;109:235–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Snook RR, Robertson A, Crudgington HS, Ritchie MG. Experimental manipulation of sexual selection and the evolution of courtship song in Drosophila pseudoobscura. Behav Genet. 2005;35(3):245–55.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Broughton SJ, Piper MD, Ikeya T, Bass TM, Jacobson J, Driege Y, Martinez P, Hafen E, Withers DJ, Leevers SJ, et al. Longer lifespan, altered metabolism, and stress resistance in Drosophila from ablation of cells making insulin-like ligands. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005;102(8):3105–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Gilchrist AS, Partridge L. Why it is difficult to model sperm displacement in Drosophila melanogaster: the relationship between sperm transfer and copulation duration. Evolution. 2000;54:534–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Manier MK, Belote JM, Berben KS, Novikov D, Stuart WT, Pitnick S. Resolving Mechanisms of Competitive Fertilization Success in Drosophila melanogaster. Science. 2010;328:354–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Abramoff MD, Magalhaes PJ, Ram SJ. Image Processing with ImageJ. Biophoton Int. 2004;11:36–42.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Fricke C, Martin OY, Bretman A, Bussière LF, Chapman T. Sperm competitive ability and indices of lifetime reproductive success. Evolution. 2010;64:2746–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Team RC. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Venables WN, Ripley BD. Modern Applied Statistics with S. 4th ed. New York: Springer; 2002.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  62. Perry JC, Joag R, Hosken DJ, Wedell N, Radwan J, Wigby S (2016) Data from: Experimental evolution under hyper-promiscuity in Drosophila melanogaster. Oxford University Research Archive. DOI: 10.5287/bodleian:Y5bAqDMnJ

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Stephen Goodwin for use of microscopy equipment, Graham Taylor for use of a microbalance, and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments that improved the manuscript.

Funding

RJ and RT-qPCR analyses were funded by the Foundation for Polish Science (FNP) within the “Environmental Stress, Population Viability and Adaptation” project (MPD/2009-3/5) and by the Institute of Environmental Sciences, JU (DS/MND/WbiNoZ/InoS/15/2013). JCP was supported by fellowships from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (Canada) and Jesus College, Oxford. SW was supported by a grant from the Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour, fellowships from Lloyds Tercentenary Research Foundation, NERC (NE/J018937/1) and the BBSRC (BB/K014544/1), and a Welcome Trust VIP award.

Availability of data and materials

The data sets supporting the results of this article are available in the Oxford University Research Archive (ORA-Data) repository (DOI: 10.5287/bodleian:Y5bAqDMnJ; reference [62]).

Authors’ contributions

SW designed the experimental evolution. JCP and SW designed and conducted the behavioural and morphological experiments. RJ, DJH, NW and JR designed the gene expression assay and RJ conducted it. All authors contributed to manuscript writing. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jennifer C. Perry.

Additional files

Additional file 1:

Supporting results. Courtship behaviour results for unselected SPR-, white-eyed control and wild-type control males. Table S1. Full statistical results for latency until mating in the sperm competition experiments. Table S2. Copulation duration results in second matings. Table S3. Results for post-mating fecundity. Table S4. Full statistical results for latency until re-mating in the sperm competition experiments. Table S5. Results for male and female body mass and both absolute and size-corrected male testes and accessory gland size. Table S6. Full statistical results for proportion of offspring sired when a male was first to mate. Table S7. Full statistical results for proportion of offspring sired when a male was second to mate. Table S8. Details of the RT-qPCR calibration curve. Table S9. Details of primer characteristics. (DOCX 67 kb)

Additional file 2: Figure S1.

The backcrossing procedure used to generate the experimental populations. Figure S2. The backcrossing procedure used to generate SPR+ evolved controls for the sperm competition experiments. Figure S3. The backcrossing procedure used to generate SPR- evolved controls for the sperm competition experiments. (PPT 368 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Perry, J.C., Joag, R., Hosken, D.J. et al. Experimental evolution under hyper-promiscuity in Drosophila melanogaster . BMC Evol Biol 16, 131 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-016-0699-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-016-0699-8

Keywords