- Research article
- Open access
- Published:
Selection for small body size favours contrasting sex-specific life histories, boldness and feeding in medaka, Oryzias latipes
BMC Evolutionary Biology volume 19, Article number: 127 (2019)
Abstract
Background
Studying variation in life-history traits and correlated behaviours, such as boldness and foraging (i.e., pace-of-life syndrome), allows us to better understand how these traits evolve in a changing environment. In fish, it is particularly relevant studying the interplay of resource abundance and size-selection. These are two environmental stressors affecting fish in natural conditions, but also associated with human-induced environmental change. For instance, fishing, one of the most important threats for freshwater and marine populations, results in both higher mortality on large-sized fish and reduced population density.
Results
Medaka, Oryzias latipes, from lines selected for large or small size over ten generations, were exposed individually to high or low food availability from birth to adulthood. Maturation schedules, reproductive investment, growth, boldness and feeding were assessed to evaluate the effect of size-selection on the pace of life, and whether it differed between food contexts (high and low). Different food abundance and size-selection resulted in diverse life histories associated with different feeding and boldness behaviour, thus showing different pace-of-life-syndromes. High availability of food favoured faster growth, earlier maturation and increased shyness. Selection for small size led to slower growth in both males and females. But, the life-history trajectory to reach such growth was sex- and food-specific. Under low food conditions, females selected for small size showed earlier maturation, which led to slower adult growth and subsequent low willingness to feed, compared to females selected for large size. No line differences were found in females at high food conditions. In contrast, males exposed to selection for small size grew slower both as juvenile and adult, and were bolder under both feeding regimes. Therefore, the response to size-selection was more sensitive to food availability in females than in males.
Conclusions
We showed that size-selection (over ten generations) and resource abundance (over developmental time) led to changes in life history and behaviour. However, the effect of size-selection was sex- and context-specific, calling for precaution when drawing general conclusions on the population-level effects (or lack of them) of size-selective fishing. Conservation and management plans should consider this sex- and context-specificity.
Background
Variation in life histories arises from different trajectories of survival, growth and fecundity that maximises fitness in different environments [1]. Somatic growth, time of maturation and reproductive investment are key factors in an individual’s life cycle and hence represent a life-history strategy [2,3,4]. Numerous environmental factors (e.g., resource abundance, temperature, inter- and intraspecific interactions) can affect growth and maturation [5, 6]. Studying how these factors affect variation in life-history strategies is particularly relevant to better understand the potential evolutionary adaptation of wild populations [7, 8].
In the particular case of fish, size-selective mortality and resource availability are two environmental stressors of interest. Selection on body size in fish can result from natural mortality, as predators normally target small individuals [9,10,11] or from fishing-induced mortality that is commonly higher on large individuals [12,13,14]. Food abundance is affected by many factors such as intraspecific competition, climate change and predation [15]. Increased predation (by predators or fishing) not only leads to selection on size, but further results in lower abundance in the population and thus increases food availability for the survivors [16]. This interplay between size-selection and resource availability on life-history traits is still poorly understood [17].
Size-selective mortality on large individuals, as that induced by fishing, leads to decreased life span. And hence, according to life-history theory, fast life histories are favoured – i.e., earlier maturation and increased reproductive investment [5, 14, 18, 19]. However, how size-selective fishing affects juvenile and adult growth remains unclear [5, 14]. Most empirical evidence shows no direct effect of size-selective fishing on growth [20], or an indirect effect resulting in reduced growth only after maturation [14]. Yet, such a response may not appear if resource availability is high [21]. Moreover, populations exposed to fishing would experience fast or slow growth, depending on the selectivity of the gear and the minimum size imposed [5, 22, 23].
Life-history diversity often entails co-variation with behaviour, as behaviour and physiology are the basis for the trade-off between current and future reproduction (referred as pace-of-life syndrome) [18, 24]. Fast life history is expected to be linked with behaviours that favour energy acquisition (e.g., foraging behaviour) and reproduction over survival (e.g., boldness), resulting in a fast pace of life [18, 24]. However, the few studies that have evaluated how behaviour was affected by size-selective mortality showed that fish exposed to positive size selection were less bold and less eager to forage [25, 26]. Behavioural and life-history co-variation are often sex- and context-dependent [24, 27]. Thus, knowledge on how the suit of life-history and behavioural traits changes due to size-selective mortality is still limited. Particularly, little is known about whether such changes are affected by the release from density-dependent food limitation following the reduction in abundance in harvested populations, and whether both sexes despite their different investment in reproduction are responding in a similar way.
Here we tested how two different size-selective mortality regimes (on large or small size) affected the pace-of-life syndrome in medaka (Oryzias latipes). In addition, we evaluated whether the result of size-selection differed under different food availability conditions. Wild medaka were used to produce two laboratory-reared lineages over ten generations with selection for either large or small standard body length at 75 days post-hatching (dph); here referred to as large-selected and small-selected lines, respectively [28]. The line selected for small size mimics the size-selective pressure induced by fishing, where large individuals are harvested and mainly the smaller individuals can reproduce. The large-selected line mimics natural size-selection, where predators feed on small individuals. Lines were selected under common-garden conditions with abundant food supply. Individuals from the 11th generation were reared in isolation under two different feeding regimes (high and low food availability). We fitted data on age and length to a biphasic (juvenile and adult) growth model that allowed studying trade-offs in energy allocation between growth, maturation and reproduction [29,30,31]. Females were measured every 2 weeks, while males every three, thus models were fitted separately for each sex. Once fish became mature, we assessed the feeding behaviour and boldness of each individual. Both behaviours have ecological validity, as they are respectively linked to foraging rate and risk of predation and hence related to fitness [32].
We expected individuals from the small-selected line to express 1) fast life histories (i.e., early maturation and high investment in reproduction), with growth only slowing down after maturation, and 2) behaviours associated with a fast pace of life, i.e., higher boldness and feeding rate compared to large-selected line [14, 18]. We also predicted that 3) individuals under low food availability would present slower pace of life relative to under high food [2, 33]. The effects of size-selection on behavioural and life-history traits would hold true at differing food abundances and for both sexes if food availability did not play a role in the selection process [17, 34], as expected under controlled equal and abundant food availability in the selection experiment. However, different reproductive investments between sexes may affect this expectation [27].
Results
Life-history traits
Both male and female medaka showed large inter-individual variation in growth and life-history parameters (age at maturation and maximal potential growth; Figs. 1a and b). Life-history traits differed between the two laboratory-reared lines (i.e., small-selected vs. large-selected) and the two food availability conditions applied during development (high vs. low; see Methods for details). Both low food availability and selection for small size led to smaller size, but the life-history trajectory leading there was context-specific (different between treatments) and different between sexes.
At the end of experiment (180 dph), fish in the high food availability treatment were larger compared to those fed low food in both sexes (Figs. 1a and b). In addition, fish from the large-selected line were larger at 180 dph compared to those originating from the small-selected line, although this was food-dependent in females (Fig. 1a). Overall, the effect of size-selection in females was absent in the juvenile phase of the growth curve (Fig. 1a). It was only observable in adult growth under low food conditions, when the earlier age at maturation of small-selected females resulted in a slower adult growth and hence smaller length at 180 dph (Fig. 1a; Table 1). Thus, the effect of line on female growth only occurred indirectly through its effect on maturation. The effect of size-selective mortality in males was evident, with juvenile and adult growth always being slower for small-selected males, and direct, as it was not affected through any other parameter.
Average age at maturation (amat) was 68 dph (SD = 24.5) in females and 71 dph (SD = 10.6) in males, which is within the range commonly observed for medaka (60–90 days) [35]. It was affected by the interaction of food and line in females, but not in males. As expected, females delayed maturation under low food conditions, but the delay differed between small- and large-selected females (Tables 1 and 2). At low food conditions, small-selected females matured earlier (81 dph) than large-selected females (97 dph), while at high food both lines matured at 48 dph (Tables 1 and 2). In contrast, maturation in males was not affected by size-selection, and only delayed under low food (Tables 3 and 4). Investment in reproduction was not affected by size-selection in either sex, as lower food led to a lower investment in reproduction in both sexes (Tables 2 and 4).
The maximal potential growth rate (related to β; see Methods) describes the growth rate before the investment in reproduction is accounted for [29]. Higher values of β correspond to faster juvenile somatic growth rate, i.e., before any investment in reproduction takes place. Specifically, β is the exponent of the allometric relationship between growth rate and weight. It describes the allometric scaling of energy readily available to growth (i.e., net energy after expenditure on metabolism has been accounted for) [29]. Our average estimated value for β was 0.58 (SD = 0.06), which is similar to the 0.66 assumed in many growth models [29, 36]. For both males and females, β was lower – resulting in a slower juvenile growth – at low food conditions than at high food conditions, (Tables 1 and 3). In males there was also a direct effect of line on β, which was additive to the effect of food on β, but lower in magnitude. Large-selected males at high food conditions presented the highest value of β and hence the fastest juvenile growth rate, while small-selected males at low food had the lowest β and slowest juvenile growth rate (Table 3; Fig. 1b).
Behavioural traits
Two different behaviours were assessed once the test fish reached maturity. Feeding behaviour was measured as the total number of bites the fish took at the supplied food, while boldness was related to time spent freezing at the bottom of the tank – the shorter freezing time the bolder an individual is [32]. Both behaviours were repeatable over time and across contexts (Additional file 1: Table S1). Feeding behaviour and boldness were evaluated as both total amount (bites on food or freezing time) and probability (of zero bites or zero freezing) due to the nature of the data (see Methods for details). Food availability and size-selection affected the behaviours and this was sex-specific (Table 5).
Feeding rate was higher under low food conditions in both sexes (Fig. 2), but this effect was stronger in females than in males. Females reared at low food fed more than those at high food, as indicated by both their higher number of total food bites (Fig. 2b; low-high food comparison, estimate ± SE = 0.27 ± 0.10 bites, z = 2.72, P = 0.007) and lower probability to remain without eating (low-high food comparison: 3 times lower odds, z = − 4.48, P < 0.0001). Males at low food had only lower probability of no biting at all relative to high food males (low-high food comparison: 1.6 times less odds, z = − 2.81, P = 0.005). In addition, small-selected females presented reduced feeding activity (Fig. 2a–c) and they had higher probability of not eating at all compared to large-selected ones (small- large-selected comparison: 2.4 higher odds, z = 3.48, P < 0.001).
Finally, total freezing time (used as a proxy for boldness) was affected by size-selection in males and by food availability in females (Fig. 3). Females at low food conditions were bolder (Fig. 3a–c) than females fed high food quantities, as the former had 1.6 times higher probability of not freezing at all (estimate ± SE = 0.48 ± 0.21 log(odds), z = 2.24, P = 0.025). Small-selected males were bolder – they froze 0.43 s less than males selected for large size (SE = 0.15, z = − 2.97, P = 0.003), hence they had higher probability of not freezing at all (Fig. 3 d–f; 1.8 higher odds, z = 3.50, P = 0.0005) relative to large-selected males.
Discussion
Size-selection over ten generations affected life-history traits, boldness, and feeding behaviour. These traits were also affected by exposure to different food availabilities during development of each fish to a somewhat larger extent. Both the selection for small size (experienced by the parental generation) of fish and exposure to low food availability during development, led to shorter length at adulthood. However, the life-history traits (age at maturation, maximal potential growth and investment in reproduction) that led to such outcome were context-specific and differed between food availability and size-selection. Moreover, the effect of size-selection, but not the effect of food availability, was sex-specific.
It should be noted that comparisons between the two drivers evaluated here (i.e. size-selection and food availability) are challenging, as they were acting on different ecological processes (i.e. plasticity vs. evolution). Specifically, food availability affected the environment of the studied fish during their development from juvenile to adulthood, while size-selection was the stressor experienced by ten ancestral generations. Food availability and size-selection can lead also to evolutionary and plastic changes, respectively [33, 37], but here the experimental design limited those effects. Indeed, food availability was a proximate driver of plastic change in life-history traits, while size-selection could affect life history through genetic change (evolution), as it was performed under common garden conditions over ten generations [38,39,40]. Epigenetic inheritance cannot be ruled out, but it is likely not related to size-selection [40]. We warrant this simplification to better disentangle the effect of each treatment, before more complex interactions between treatments, through intraspecific competition should be assessed. In addition, sex differences could be due to differences in handling the sexes during the experiment due to time and space limitations. Males were measured every 3 weeks and water quality was maintained with partial water changes (for half of the males) and constant flow-through (on the other half), while females were measured every 2 weeks and water quality was maintained by constant flow-through of water. Even though these differences should not affect the effect of experimental treatments, but the statistical power, its effect cannot be completely ruled out.
Effect of food
As expected, low food availability led to smaller size at adulthood and slower growth relative to high food availability in both sexes [2, 33, 41] . Low food availability led to higher willingness to feed and increased boldness (less freezing time) compared to high food availability, although the latter only happened in females. Thus, the hunger level might be the driver of increased boldness and increased willingness to eat, as seen elsewhere [42, 43]. Specifically, Magnuson [43] found that under limited food medaka became more aggressive to ensure access to food and maximal growth rate. Fast growth is generally associated with boldness and willingness to forage through evolutionary trade-offs [18], but these links often depend on environmental conditions [44]. In the present study, fast-growing fish at high food condition might have been feeding at a maximum ratio. Indeed, they showed lower immediate willingness or need to feed (no hunger and low appetite) during our observational assays, and lower boldness (at least in females). The links here between growth and behaviour are driven by hunger levels and are thus probably plastic.
Effect of size-selection
Fish selected for small body size grew slower relative to those selected for large body size, as normally observed in other size-selective experiments on fish [25, 38, 39, 45, 46]. However, the overall shorter length at adulthood in small-selected fish, were achieved through different growth trajectories between sexes. Here, growth depended on the combination of the exponent of the maximal potential growth rate (related to β) and age at maturation.
Size-selection affected growth rates in males throughout their lives, but only during adulthood in females – this was due to size-selection effects on different parameters. Males experienced a direct effect of size-selection on growth, affecting only β – males selected for small size had lower values of β and thus grew slower. This, together with the lack of differences in maturation and reproductive investment between lines, led to different growth curves throughout the life of the fish. Females experienced a direct effect of size-selection on age at maturation under low food conditions. This led to an indirect effect of size-selection on growth, which resulted in equal juvenile growth between lines followed by a slowed down adult growth in small-selected females reared at low food. At high food conditions, females of both lines presented equal growth curves. Reduced age at maturation after selection for small size is expected from theory [14, 19] and observed in laboratory experiments [25, 34, 39, 45], but only occurred for females reared under low food conditions in our experiment.
The interaction between size-selection and food suggests that food availability played a role during the selection process in females [17, 34]. The higher investment in reproduction in females relative to in males may have resulted in females perceiving their food environment, during the selection process, as quantitatively low. This higher sensitivity to food in females also became apparent in our results on behaviour – the feeding and boldness of females were affected more by food than those of males. This is in concordance with earlier studies in medaka showing that females are more sensitive to fasting than males, resulting in reduced gonadosomatic index and fecundity [47]. Thus, it seems that females were only selected for small size under low food availability, and hence the response to selection is stronger under conditions similar to those experienced under the selection process [48]. Similar resource-dependent response to size-selection has been seen in other fish species [17, 49]. In the case of the killifish, Rivulus hartii, the females response to size-selection were also more sensitive to food availability than the males [17]. The context-specificity of life-history trajectories, which differed between sexes here, but also between species [25, 38, 39, 45, 46], indicate that the life history response to size-selection is more complex than often assumed.
Medaka selected for small size were bolder than medaka selected for large size. This was evident at least in males, which showed reduced juvenile and adult growth. Medaka females fed less when selected for small size, which presented early maturation and slow adult growth. Size-selection experiments with Atlantic silversides, Menidia menidia, have shown a reduced food consumption in fish selected for small size [26]. However, similar selection experiments have indicated reduced boldness in fish selected for small size [25, 26]. As noted earlier, the evolutionary link between fast growth and boldness is common mainly when predation is high and resources are limited [44]. For instance, in medaka exposed to low food, higher aggression (commonly correlated with boldness [32]) was linked with higher growth rate, but this link disappeared when food supply was high [43]. When resources are abundant, low activity and boldness leads to higher growth as predicted by the allocation model [50]. Overall, we observed that changes in life-history parameters due to size-selection also led to changes in behaviour, which were consistent over time and among context [51].
Medaka do not present drastic morphological sexual dimorphism [35], but males and females present behavioural and physiological differences. Both sexes present courtship and competitive behaviours, but these are more evident in males [43, 52], while females invest more in reproductive tissues [53]. Moreover, external factors (e.g., food, temperature, pollutants) seem to alter reproductive investment in females (but not behaviour) and aggressiveness in males (but not reproduction or to a lower extent) [47, 54,55,56,57]. Overall, this sexual dimorphism in sensitivity to external stressors could explain the sexual differences observed in the present study. Moreover, our results highlight the need of assessing sexual differences while evaluating life-history and behavioural traits.
Here we show that the effect of size-selection, such as the one induced by fishing, on life history can entail behavioural changes. Boldness and foraging are at the core of predator-prey interactions, as they determine the effects of consumers on their prey and are affected by the presence of predators [58]. Therefore, changes in behaviour due to human-induced size-selection can in turn affect the resource community and ecosystem processes through different pathways, such as alteration of the strength of the trophic cascade [59]. Moreover, the links between life history and behaviour are more complex than often assumed and dependent on sex and environmental conditions. A better understanding on how size-selective mortality affects this suit of traits is not only relevant for management and conservation of exploited species, but allow us to predict further ecosystem consequences. We suggest that future mesocosm-based experiments should assess whether differences in these size-dependent correlated traits can translate to changes in the trophic cascade to better evaluate the ecosystem impacts of size-dependent mortality. Moreover, such experiments could allow the interplay of food availability and size-selection and hence assess the context-dependency of such ecological variations.
Conclusions
In the present study, both sexes displayed smaller size and slower growth when exposed to selection for small size, similar to the size-selection induced by fishing. However, life-history strategy and pace-of-life syndromes were sex- and context-specific in the present study.
Small-selected females showed a fast pace of life under low food conditions, i.e., early maturation and fast juvenile growth – only slowed down after maturation. Slower adult growth was linked to reduced feeding rate. However, small-selected males grew slower throughout life linked with a higher boldness relative to fish selected for large size. Sex differences may be due to differences in investment in reproduction and food requirements during the selection process.
Conservation plans concerned with size-selectivity (e.g., fishing or introduction of novel predators) should consider that a suit of behavioural and life-history traits, rather than only size, are responding to the new selection. In addition, the interplay between size-selection and resource availability should be evaluated to better account for the impact on ecosystem functioning and services.
Methods
Selection lines
Individuals used in this study were the offspring of the 10th generation (F10) produced by a size-selection experiment performed in the laboratory (see [28] for details on experimental protocol). Briefly, the selection for small standard length (SL ± 1 mm) (referred as Small-selected line) mimicked the selection imposed by fishing, where large individuals are removed and only small individuals are allowed to breed. Selection for large individuals (i.e., large SL, referred as Large-selected line) represents natural mortality in the wild. Specifically, at 60 days post hatching (dph), the ten brother-sister families with the shortest or largest average length were selected for the small-selected and large-selected lines, respectively. At 75 dph, individual size-selection took place and the breeders (two males and two females per family) for the next generation were chosen. The largest and the smallest individuals per family were chosen for the line selected for large size and selected for small size, respectively.
Fish rearing and feeding experiment
At generation F10, we randomly chose five families from each line to produce at least 64 fish per line and per feeding treatment (N = 256). F10 breeders were 90 dph when eggs were collected and kept in an incubator until the larvae hatched. The incubator was checked for daily hatchlings and hatching day was recorded for all larvae. Larvae hatched on the same day were kept together within family in 3 L tanks for 2 weeks. At 14 dph, each surviving individual larva was randomly assigned to one of two feeding regimes and housed in isolation in 1 L tanks.
Every 2 weeks females were measured for SL with a measuring board and weighted (W ± 0.001 g), while males were only measured and weighted every 3 weeks due to time limitation. The first two measurements (at 14 and 28 dph) were obtained with ImageJ (version 1.51 s; [60]) from photographs of the larvae placed in a petri dish filled with water (no weight was taken), as the larvae were too small to be handled otherwise. Later, individuals were anaesthetised (Metacaine; Sigma; following [35] protocols) to minimise stress during handling. At the end of the experiment 259 fish were included in the growth analysis as they had at least four measurements of size. Total number was not balanced among sexes and treatments. Specifically, we obtained growth trajectories from a total of 143 females (39 from large-selected line in high food, 36 from small-selected line in high food, 28 from large-selected line in low food and 40 from small-selected line in low food) and 116 males (19 from large-selected line in high food, 41 from small-selected line in high food, 20 from large-selected line in low food, and 36 from small-selected line in low food).
Fish were fed once a day (morning) with quantified amounts of newly hatched Artemia salina based on previous experiments [61, 62] During the first 2 weeks, from birth until the initiation of the treatments, all fish received the same food quantities (0–14 dph: 0.01 ml of filtered, undiluted Artemia per fish). Then, high and low food levels were applied and increased every 2 weeks. These quantities of food delivery were chosen to sustain two different growth rates, with the high level being double the low level [61, 62]. Specifically, high food level consisted of 0.01 mL of Artemia at 15–28 dph and reached 0.05 mL from 143 dph until the end of the experiment.
In the 1-L tanks, females were housed in a flow-through system, while only half of the tanks holding males where housed in the same flow-through system. Due to space limitations, the other half of male tanks was housed outside of the flow-through system (partial water changes every 2 weeks), but still inside the same lab (same temperature of 26 degrees Celsius). These males were only outside of the flow-through system during the growth part of the experiment, and not during the behavioural assessment. In addition, to minimise this effect, males were randomly rotated every 3 weeks between inside and outside the flow-through system. Finally, measurements were more frequent in females than males and, because they were also exposed to constant flow-through, data sets are analysed separately. At the end of the study all individuals were anaesthetised with Metacaine (Sigma; following [35] protocols) and later euthanised with an overdose of Metacaine as they were included in a study on pituitary gene expression.
Estimation of life history traits
The life-history parameters, age at maturation, investment in reproduction and growth rate were estimated using the Quince-Boukal biphasic growth model [29, 30]. This growth model fits better juvenile and adult growth curves compared to other commonly used growth models for fish, and has proven useful for generating management advice [36]. The model follows a continuous function with a smooth transition between the juvenile and adult growth phase. This transition is due to allocation of energy to reproduction. It assumes that the maximal potential growth rate scales allometrically with body size.
The formulation assumes that juveniles allocate all surplus energy into growth (i.e., the investment in reproduction ra = 0). Juvenile growth curve for length, at age, a, La, follows:
The adult growth rate considers the investment in reproduction, r, of the mature individuals, whose age is larger than their age at maturation (a > amat) and the weight-age growth curve follows:
where H = c(1 − β) b−(1 − β), R = 1/[1 + (1 − β) r], assuming the conversion factor between somatic and gonadic investment, q, in [29] to be 1 as in [36, 63]. L0 is length at birth, c and β are the intercept and exponent in the allometric relationship of growth rate with weight, dW/dt = c Wβ – which is also referred as maximal potential growth rate. b and α are the intercept and exponent of the allometric relationship between weight and length, W = b Lα. See [29] for all details in formulation.
The coefficient, b, and exponent, α, of the allometric relationship of weight, W, with length, L, were estimated with the data prior running the growth model. The values obtained from a regression model with log-weight and log-length were α = 2.7 and b = 0.04 mg mm-2.7, which were used for both males and females. Length at birth, L0, was obtained from a subsample of the individuals (25 females and 22 males) that were photographed and measured at 0 dph. Length at birth did not differ between sexes (F44, 1 = 0.67, P = 0.5) or lines (F44, 1 = 0.36, P = 0.7). The mean length at 0 dph was 3.9 ± 0.4 mm, thus L0 = 4.0 mm was used in the model for both males and females. The linear models were performed with the “stats” R package [64].
Growth curves were estimated separately for males and females, as sexes normally differ in their life-history optima and considering together might impede the study of pace-of life syndrome [27], but also due to the differences in experimental handling between sexes. Thus, here we aimed at studying the effect of size-selection on growth in both sexes without directly comparing sexes. For each sex, the growth model estimated age at maturation amat, reproductive investment r, and the exponent in the allometric growth rate-weight relationship β. To improve model convergence the coefficient in the allometric growth rate-weight relationship was fixed to c = 0.15 mg1- β day− 1. Initial exploration of the data showed that this value was the most appropriate for our data and changes in this value with an increase or decrease of 10% did not qualitatively change the results. This scaling coefficient is species-specific [65], it has been estimated for another small freshwater fish, guppy Poecilia reticulata (c = 9–14 mg1- β day− 1; [45]).
All statistical analyses were performed with the R software (version 3.5.0; [64]). The parameters were estimated from a non-linear mixed effect model in the R package “nlme” (version 3.1.137; [66]) with fish identity as random effect on r and β for males and on β only for females. The random structure was chosen following recommendations from [67]. Models included a residual autocorrelation structure ARMA (0,2), chosen according to guidelines in [67]. Line, food treatment and their interaction were tested as fixed effects on amat, r, and β for both sexes separately. All possible models were run and model selection was done by comparing them with AIC (Akaike information criterion). The model yielding the lowest AIC was considered the best-ranked model in the Kullback–Leibler information [68]. However, for males this model was further simplified through hypothesis testing and non-significant predictors were dropped one by one to obtain a more parsimonious model [67].
Behavioural traits
Behavioural observations took place on mature fish (mean age = 124 dph ± 33 SD for females and 131 dph ± 32 sd for males) in three different settings: 1) Control conditions where fish were fed Artemia undisturbed, 2) Novel conditions where fish were fed a novel food source (4 pellets; JBL NovoGrano Mix) undisturbed, and 3) Threatening conditions where fish were fed Artemia immediately after being netted out of the water for 2 s as threat stimuli. Each fish was exposed to the three conditions inside their tank in a randomized order, each exposition replicated twice. Only one condition was tested per day and the second replicate took place 1 week after the first one. The six measurements (three conditions repeated twice) of each behaviour (i.e. feeding and boldness; see details below) ensured that we considered consistent intrinsic individual variation in behaviour [51]. However, the effect of time and experimental condition was not the main focus of the study, thus details on the experimental set up and across-context repeatability can be found in the supplementary material (Additional file 1: Tables S1–S3).
During 5 min of observations we counted 1) number of bites to the supplied food (Artemia or pellets) as a measurement of willingness to feed, and thus is associated with foraging on prey [26], and 2) total time frozen at the bottom of the tank, as a proxy of boldness, which is related to predation risk and survival in natural conditions [32]. A fish that spends more time immobile is considered less bold than one that spends little time immobile [32]. Both behaviours are ecologically relevant, as they are linked to interactions with prey and predators [58]. Behaviour tests were performed on 80 females and 107 males, as only these were available at the time.
The two behaviours were analysed with generalized mixed effect models using the R package “glmmTMB” [69] with measurement nested within fish identity and experimental condition included as two separate random effect on model intercept. Full models contained size-selection (Large-selected vs. Small-selected), food (High food vs. Low food) and their interaction as fixed effects. The analyses of number of bites and freezing time (measured as an integer count of seconds) were performed following negative binomial distribution and allowing for zero inflation, as 33 and 49% of the data for number of bites were zeros for females and males respectively, and 52% of the values for freezing time were zeros for both males and females. Negative binomial distribution was used due to overdispersion observed with Poisson distribution. The final model was the one ranked with the lowest AIC. The residuals from all the final models were evaluated following [67] and fulfilled all the requirements. In addition, simulations showed that the zero-inflated models represented well the data; particularly the models estimated an equivalent number of zeros as in the original data on average in 40% of the simulations [70]. It should be noted that the analyses with negative binomial distribution and zero inflation evaluate each behaviour in two ways: 1) total amount of bites or seconds frozen, and 2) probability of zero bites or zero seconds frozen. Thus, the effects of size-selection and food are evaluated for both cases.
Availability of data and materials
The datasets supporting the conclusions of this article are available in the figshare repository: doi: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.8255873.v2 [71].
Abbreviations
- AIC:
-
Akaike information criterion
- a mat :
-
age at maturation
- b :
-
Intercept of the allometric relationship between weight and length
- c :
-
Intercept of the allometric relationship between growth rate and weight
- dph:
-
Days post-hatching
- F10:
-
10th generation
- L:
-
Length
- L0 :
-
Length at birth
- r :
-
reproductive investment
- SL:
-
Standard length
- W:
-
Weight
- α:
-
Exponent of the allometric relationship between weight and length
- β:
-
Exponent of the allometric relationship between growth rate and weight
References
Stearns SC. The evolution of life histories. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1992.
Stearns SC. Life history evolution: successes, limitations, and prospects. Naturwissenschaften. 2000;87:476–86.
Roff DA. The evolution of life histories. 1st ed. New York: Chapman & Hall; 1992.
Stearns SC, Koella JC. The evolution of phenotypic plasticity in life-history traits: predictions of reaction norms for age and size at maturity. Evolution. 1986;40:893–913.
Enberg K, Jørgensen C, Dunlop ES, Varpe Ø, Boukal DS, Baulier L, et al. Fishing-induced evolution of growth: concepts, mechanisms and the empirical evidence. Mar Ecol. 2012;33:1–25.
Berner D, Blanckenhorn WU. An ontogenetic perspective on the relationship between age and size at maturity. Funct Ecol. 2007;21:505–12.
Vitousek PM, Mooney HA, Lubchenco J, Melillo JM. Human domination of Earth’s. Ecosystems Science. 1997;277:494–9.
Halpern BS, Walbridge S, Selkoe KA, Kappel CV, Micheli F, D’Agrosa C, et al. A global map of human impact on marine ecosystems. Science. 2008;319:948–52.
Lorenzen K. The relationship between body weight and natural mortality in juvenile and adult fish: a comparison of natural ecosystems and aquaculture. J Fish Biol. 1996;49:627–42.
Sogard SM. Size-selective mortality in the juvenile stage of teleost fishes: a review. Bull Mar Sci. 1997;60:1129–57.
Gislason H, Daan N, Rice JC, Pope JG. Size, growth, temperature and the natural mortality of marine fish. Fish Fish. 2010;11:149–58.
Pauly D, Christensen V, Guénette S, Pitcher TJ, Sumaila UR, Walters CJ, et al. Towards sustainability in world fisheries. Nature. 2002;418:689–95.
Kolding J, Bundy A, van Zwieten PAM, Plank MJ. Fisheries, the inverted food pyramid. ICES J Mar Sci. 2016;73:1697–713.
Heino M, Diaz Pauli B, Dieckmann U. Fisheries-induced evolution. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 2015;46:461–80.
Hays G, Richardson A, Robinson C. Climate change and marine plankton. Trends Ecol Evol. 2005;20:337–44.
Heino M, Godø OR. Fisheries-induced selection pressures in the context of sustainable fisheries. Bull Mar Sci. 2002;70:639–56.
Walsh MR, Reznick DN. Interactions between the direct and indirect effects of predators determine life history evolution in a killifish. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2008;105:594–9.
Réale D, Garant D, Humphries MM, Bergeron P, Careau V, Montiglio P-O. Personality and the emergence of the pace-of-life syndrome concept at the population level. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2010;365:4051–63.
Charlesworth B. Evolution in age-structured populations. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1994.
Hilborn R, Minte-Vera CV. Fisheries-induced changes in growth rates in marine fisheries: are they significant? Bull Mar Sci. 2008;83:95–105.
Reznick DN, Nunney L, Tessier A. Big houses, big cars, superfleas and the costs of reproduction. Trends Ecol Evol. 2000;15:421–5.
Dunlop ES, Heino M, Dieckmann U. Eco-genetic modeling of contemporary life-history evolution. Ecol Appl. 2009;19:1815–34.
Boukal DS, Dunlop E, Heino M, Dieckmann U. Fisheries-induced evolution of body size and other life history traits: the impact of gear selectivity. ICES CM 2008/F:07. 2008.
Montiglio P-O, Dammhahn M, Dubuc Messier G, Réale D. The pace-of-life syndrome revisited: the role of ecological conditions and natural history on the slow-fast continuum. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology. 2018;72:179.
Uusi-Heikkilä S, Whiteley AR, Kuparinen A, Matsumura S, Venturelli PA, Wolter C, et al. The evolutionary legacy of size-selective harvesting extends from genes to populations. Evol Appl. 2015;8:597–620.
Walsh MR, Munch SB, Chiba S, Conover DO. Maladaptive changes in multiple traits caused by fishing: impediments to population recovery. Ecol Lett. 2006;9:142–8.
Hämäläinen A, Immonen E, Tarka M, Schuett W. Evolution of sex-specific pace-of-life syndromes: causes and consequences. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2018;72:1–15.
Renneville C, Millot A, Agostini S, Carmignac D, Maugars G, Dufour S, et al. Anthropogenic selection along directions of most evolutionary resistance. bioRxiv; 2018;498683.
Boukal DS, Dieckmann U, Enberg K, Heino M, Jørgensen C. Life-history implications of the allometric scaling of growth. J Theor Biol. 2014;359:199–207.
Quince C, Abrams PA, Shuter BJ, Lester NP. Biphasic growth in fish I: theoretical foundations. J Theor Biol. 2008;254:197–206.
Wilson KL, Honsey AE, Moe B, Venturelli P. Growing the biphasic framework: techniques and recommendations for fitting emerging growth models. Methods Ecol Evol. 2017;9:822–33.
Réale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse NJ. Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol Rev. 2007;82:291–318.
Arendt JD. Adaptive intrinsic growth rates: an integration across taxa. Q Rev Biol. 1997;72:149–77.
Reznick DN, Bryga HA. Life-history evolution in guppies (Poecilia reticulata: Poeciliidae). V. Genetic basis of parallelism in life histories. Am Nat. 1996;147:339–59.
Kinoshita M, Murata K, Naruse K, Tanaka M. Medaka. Biology, management, and experimental protocols. Ames, Iowa, USA: Wiley-Blackwell; 2009.
Minte-Vera CV, Maunder MN, Casselman JM, Campana SE. Growth functions that incorporate the cost of reproduction. Fish Res. 2016;180:31–44.
Law R. Fishing, selection, and phenotypic evolution. ICES J Mar Sci. 2000;57:659–68.
Conover DO, Munch SB. Sustaining fisheries yields over evolutionary time scales. Science. 2002;297:94–6.
van Wijk SJ, Taylor MI, Creer S, Dreyer C, Rodrigues FM, Ramnarine IW, et al. Experimental harvesting of fish populations drives genetically based shifts in body size and maturation. Front Ecol Environ. 2013;11:181–7.
Perry H. Genomic analysis of shifts in life history traits in the Trinidadian guppy, Poecilia reticulata. PhD thesis. Bangor University; 2015.
Auer SK. Phenotypic plasticity in adult life-history strategies compensates for a poor start in life in Trinidadian guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Am Nat. 2010;176:818–29.
Gotceitas V, J-GJ G. Foraging under the risk of predation in juvenile Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar L.): effects of social status and hunger. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology. 1991;29:255–61.
Magnuson JJ. An analysis of aggressive behavior, growth, and competition for food and space in medaka (Oryzias latipes (Pisces, Cyprinodontidae)). Can J Zool. 1962;40:313–63.
Adriaenssens B, Johnsson JI. Shy trout grow faster: exploring links between personality and fitness-related traits in the wild. Behav Ecol. 2011;22:135–43.
Diaz Pauli B, Kolding J, Jeyakanth G, Heino M. Effects of ambient oxygen and size-selective mortality on growth and maturation in guppies. Conservation Physiology. 2017;5:1.
Amaral IPG, Johnston IA. Experimental selection for body size at age modifies early life-history traits and muscle gene expression in adult zebrafish. J Exp Biol. 2012;215:3895–904.
Hasebe M, Kanda S, Oka Y. Female-specific glucose sensitivity of gnrh1 neurons leads to sexually dimorphic inhibition of reproduction in medaka. Endocrinology. 2016;157:4318–29.
Falconer DS, Latyszewski M. The environment in relation to selection for size in mice. J Genet. 1952;51:67–80.
Bassar RD, Lopez-Sepulcre A, Reznick DN, Travis J. Experimental evidence for density-dependent regulation and selection on Trinidadian guppy life histories. Am Nat. 2013;181:25–38.
Careau V, Thomas D, Humphries M, Réale D. Energy metabolism and animal personality. Oikos. 2008;117:641–53.
Killen SS, Adriaenssens B, Marras S, Claireaux G, Cooke SJ. Context dependency of trait repeatability and its relevance for management and conservation of fish populations. Conservation Physiology. 2016;4:cow007.
Clark L, Grant JWA. Intrasexual competition and courtship in female and male Japanese medaka, Oryzias latipes: effects of operational sex ratio and density. Anim Behav. 2010;80:707–12.
Nimrod AC, Benson WH. Reproduction and development of Japanese medaka following an early life stage exposure to xenoestrogens. Aquat Toxicol. 1998;44:141–56.
Dhillon RS, Fox MG. Growth-independent effects of temperature on age and size at maturity in Japanese medaka (Oryzias latipes). Copeia. 2004;2004:37–45.
Gray MA, Teather KL, Metcalfe CD. Reproductive success and behavior of Japanese medaka (Oryzias latipes) exposed to 4- tert -octylphenol. Environ Toxicol Chem. 1999;18:2587–94.
Weber DN, Spieler RE. Effects of the light-dark cycle and scheduled feeding on behavioral and reproductive rhythms of the cyprinodont fish, medaka, Oryzias latipes. Experientia. 1987;43:621–4.
Lee P-Y, Lin C-Y, Chen T-H. Environmentally relevant exposure of 17α-ethinylestradiol impairs spawning and reproductive behavior in the brackish medaka Oryzias melastigma. Mar Pollut Bull. 2014;85:338–43.
Moran NP, Wong BBM, Thompson RM. Weaving animal temperament into food webs: implications for biodiversity. Oikos. 2017;126:917–30.
Palkovacs EP, Moritsch MM, Contolini GM, Pelletier F. Ecology of harvest-driven trait changes and implications for ecosystem management. Front Ecol Environ. 2018;16:20–8.
Schneider CA, Rasband WS, Eliceiri KW. NIH image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nat Methods. 2012;9:671–5.
Reznick DN, Endler JA. The impact of predation on life history evolution in Trinidadian guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Evolution. 1982;36:160–77.
Reznick DN. Plasticity in age and size at maturity in male guppies (Poecilia reticulata): an experimental evaluation of alternative models of development. J Evol Biol. 1990;3:185–203.
Lester NP, Shuter BJ, Abrams PA. Interpreting the von Bertalanffy model of somatic growth in fishes: the cost of reproduction. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2004;271:1625–31.
R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. 2018.
Killen SS, Atkinson D, Glazier DS. The intraspecific scaling of metabolic rate with body mass in fishes depends on lifestyle and temperature. Ecol Lett. 2010;13:184–93.
Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D, R Core Team. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1–137. 2018.
Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Walker NJ, Saveliev AA, Smith GM. Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. New York: Springer Science+Business Media, LLC; 2009.
Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and inference: a practical information-theoretical approach. 2nd ed. New York: Springer-Verlag; 2002.
Brooks ME, Kristensen K, van Benthem KJ, Magnusson Á, Berg CW, Nielsen A, et al. glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. The R Journal. 2017;9:378–400.
Zuur AF, Ieno EN. Beginner’s Guide to Zero-inflated Models with R. first. Newburgh, UK: Highland Statistics Ltd.; 2016.
Diaz Pauli B, Garric S, Evangelista C, Vøllestad LA, Edeline E. Data from selection for small body size favours contrasting sex-specific life histories, boldness and feeding in medaka, Oryzias latipes. Figshare data repository. 2019. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.8255873.v2
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Clémentine Renneville for creating the selection lines, Pauline Millot for great help in the lab, Ian Mayer for help and support with designing the behavioural observations, and David Carmignac and Romain Péronnet for their help maintaining the fish. In addition, we are grateful to Arnaud Le Rouzic and two anonymous reviewers for their comments in earlier drafts.
Funding
The Research Council of Norway, through the Marinforsk programme (project number 255601/E40) supported this work. In addition, BDP was supported by the mobility programme from the Research Council of Norway (project number 268218/MO). The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
BDP, AV and EE designed the study. BDP, EE, SG and CE maintained the fish and performed data collection. BDP with contribution of AV made final analyses. BDP wrote the first draft of the manuscript and EE, CE and AV contributed critically to the preparation of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval
Experiments were carried out in the Aquatic Platform (Plateforme aquatique, Animalerie) of the University Pierre et Marie Curie, Paris, France. The protocols used in this study were designed to minimize discomfort, distress and pain of animals. Protocols followed the guidelines and were approved by the Darwin Ethical committee (Comité d’éthique en expérimentation animale Charles Darwin; case file #Ce5/2010/041) from the French Ministry of Education, Higher Education and Research, which complies with the European Committee Guidelines on the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. In addition, specific guidelines for standard care of medaka from [35] were followed.
The medaka used in the present study are descendants of a 10 generations size-selection experiment developed at CEREEP - Ecotron IleDeFrance service unit, Foljuif, France (see [28] for details in protocol). Those lines were created from a stock of Toyohashi medaka colony. Medaka used in this study were transported as eggs to the University Pierre et Marie Curie, instead of hatched individuals to minimise stress during transportation. No special permit was necessary to transport medaka eggs between the two partner animal facilities.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Additional file
Additional file 1:
Detailed experimental set up for the behavioural assessment. Behavioural repeatability (Table S1) and effect of experimental conditions on male (Table S2) and female (Table S3) behaviours. (DOCX 32 kb)
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
About this article
Cite this article
Diaz Pauli, B., Garric, S., Evangelista, C. et al. Selection for small body size favours contrasting sex-specific life histories, boldness and feeding in medaka, Oryzias latipes. BMC Evol Biol 19, 127 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-019-1460-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-019-1460-x